全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3510篇 |
免费 | 49篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
3561篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 25篇 |
2023年 | 42篇 |
2022年 | 35篇 |
2021年 | 52篇 |
2020年 | 86篇 |
2019年 | 104篇 |
2018年 | 138篇 |
2017年 | 141篇 |
2016年 | 128篇 |
2015年 | 98篇 |
2014年 | 117篇 |
2013年 | 372篇 |
2012年 | 170篇 |
2011年 | 200篇 |
2010年 | 101篇 |
2009年 | 111篇 |
2008年 | 171篇 |
2007年 | 195篇 |
2006年 | 150篇 |
2005年 | 122篇 |
2004年 | 134篇 |
2003年 | 116篇 |
2002年 | 115篇 |
2001年 | 60篇 |
2000年 | 46篇 |
1999年 | 39篇 |
1998年 | 37篇 |
1997年 | 33篇 |
1996年 | 34篇 |
1995年 | 33篇 |
1994年 | 30篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 21篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 27篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 24篇 |
1987年 | 22篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有3561条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Anneliese A. Singh Sylvia C. Nassar Patricia Arredondo Rebecca Toporek 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2020,98(3):238-252
The authors describe the ways in which the Multicultural and Social Justice Counseling Competencies (MSJCC; Ratts, Singh, Nassar-McMillan, Butler, & McCullough, 2015) can be viewed from a human rights framework and as the latest iteration in the long history of the multicultural and social justice counseling competency movement. MSJCC implementation and integration are explored, and recommendations for innovating the MSJCC are described. 相似文献
82.
Nathan Ryan Nina Westera Mark Kebbell Rebecca Milne Mark Harrison 《Applied cognitive psychology》2020,34(3):565-576
Missing body homicide cases have gained public interest globally due to some high-profile cases. In many of these cases, the task of locating the victim's remains relies on the information investigators can gain through the interviewing of willing suspects. To date, investigative interviewing research has largely focused on the retrieval of episodic memory (events) without focusing on spatial memory, a prominent cognitive task required in locating a victim's remains. The current experiment tests the enhanced cognitive interview (ECI) against a free recall strategy in a mock homicide scenario where participants are required to hide and retrieve an object in a natural bushland setting. The results showed that those in the ECI condition produced more coarse- and fine-grained details of landmarks and their actions at and journeying to the deposition site. This demonstrates the value of using the ECI in generating more valuable information to assist in successive search attempts. 相似文献
83.
Roel Boon Rebecca Milne Eveline Rosloot Joris Heinsbroek 《Applied cognitive psychology》2020,34(5):1133-1142
Attaining an uninterrupted free report (FR) is at the heart of best practice investigative interviewing guidance. However, witnesses/victims do not naturally provide detailed accounts. Techniques have been developed to counter-act this, such as a report everything (RE) instruction. This research examined the relative effectiveness of an addition to the RE component, a demonstration of the level of detail, using a behavioural exemplar, by describing an innocuous object; the Demonstration for More detail (DeMo) technique. Participants (N = 61) watched a mock crime video and asked to recall it using one of three instructions: (a) basic FR as the control, (b) RE instruction and (c) RE instruction plus DeMo technique. Participants who were given the DeMo technique recalled more details than both the control and RE groups. Accuracy rates were similarly high across all conditions. The implications of using the DeMo technique within an investigative interview are discussed. 相似文献
84.
Deane E. Aikins Robert H. Pietrzak Joseph C. Geraci Todd Benham Paul Morrissey Steven M. Southwick 《Military psychology》2020,32(5):419-427
ABSTRACT Low treatment utilization in Soldiers with combat-related Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is an ongoing issue. The critical concern is to better understand factors which prohibit a Soldier with PTSD who wants help from seeking treatment (an “inclined abstainer”). A total of 537 Active Duty Soldiers on a US Army post completed a brief survey comprising psychometrically validated measures of stigma, behavioral health treatment beliefs, resilience, PTSD symptoms, and treatment intentions. Health-care records were prospectively tracked for 12 months to determine the relation between survey answers and treatment utilization. Sixty-three percent of those who acknowledged having a mental health-related problem did not seek help within a one-year period. Greater severity of PTSD symptoms was associated with an increased likelihood of behavioral health engagement. Soldiers that were classified as “inclined abstainers” were also more likely to endorse negative beliefs about psychotherapy and report higher levels of resilience as compared to “inclined actors.” These results suggest that a treatment model of PTSD emphasizing self-efficacy and self-reliance, while addressing negative beliefs about psychotherapy, may help promote engagement of behavioral health services among Active Duty Soldiers. 相似文献
85.
Parental involvement in their adolescents’ education plays an important role in promoting their children's academic outcomes. Yet, more research is needed to examine the relationship between parenting practices and parental warmth as well as to consider the potential joint contribution of warmth from both fathers and mothers. Thus, the primary purpose of the current study is to examine the extent to which patterns of parental warmth across fathers and mothers moderate the association between parental involvement and adolescents’ grade point average (GPA) and school engagement behaviors. Latent profile analysis was conducted to identify disparate profiles of fathers’ and mothers’ warmth within a nationally representative sample of 2,306 youths (51% male; mean age = 15.31 years, SD = 1.50; 77% non-Hispanic White) residing in opposite-sex, two-parent families from Wave I and II of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health. Latent-class enumeration processes support a five-profile solution characterized by differences in levels of parental warmth and congruency across parents: (a) Congruent High Warmth, (b) Congruent Moderate Warmth, (c) Congruent Low Warmth, (d) Incongruent High Mother/Low Father Warmth, and (e) Incongruent Low Father/Lower Mother Warmth. Subsequent multiple linear regression analyses reveal a moderating effect for Congruent Low Warmth on the relationship between parental involvement and adolescents’ GPA. Ultimately, the results show that variation in parental warmth exists across fathers and mothers with differing impact on adolescents’ outcomes. Excluding one parent without considering the joint effects of both parents will not produce an accurate and precise understanding of parenting in research or practice. 相似文献
86.
Katherine E. Gentry Rebecca N. Lewis Hunter Glanz Pedro I. Simes rpd S. Nyri Michael S. Reichert 《Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science》2020,11(5)
The multifaceted ability to produce, transmit, receive, and respond to acoustic signals is widespread in animals and forms the basis of the interdisciplinary science of bioacoustics. Bioacoustics research methods, including sound recording and playback experiments, are applicable in cognitive research that centers around the processing of information from the acoustic environment. We provide an overview of bioacoustics techniques in the context of cognitive studies and make the case for the importance of bioacoustics in the study of cognition by outlining some of the major cognitive processes in which acoustic signals are involved. We also describe key considerations associated with the recording of sound and its use in cognitive applications. Based on these considerations, we provide a set of recommendations for best practices in the recording and use of acoustic signals in cognitive studies. Our aim is to demonstrate that acoustic recordings and stimuli are valuable tools for cognitive researchers when used appropriately. In doing so, we hope to stimulate opportunities for innovative cognitive research that incorporates robust recording protocols. This article is categorized under:
- Neuroscience > Cognition
- Psychology > Theory and Methods
- Neuroscience > Behavior
- Neuroscience > Cognition
87.
Carmen E. Westerberg Nathan Wofford Safia Menssor Bryant P. Reininger Rebecca G. Deason 《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2020,32(7):587-597
ABSTRACT Faces belonging to the same category as the perceiver are better recognised than faces from different categories when tested immediately. After a delay, the same-category benefit persists and response bias becomes more liberal for other-category but not same-category faces. These effects are typically attributed to better encoding of same-category than other-category faces. To determine whether bias effects after a delay persist when immediate accuracy differences are minimised, Hispanic and Caucasian participants were given more study time for other-category than same-category faces. Memory was tested immediately and after a two-day delay. Discrimination accuracy was higher for other-category than for same-category faces during both test sessions. Bias was equivalent immediately, but after the delay, bias was more conservative for same-category than for other-category faces, especially in Caucasian participants. These results suggest that post-encoding processes differentially influence face memories from different categories, which may have implications for evaluating face memory in everyday situations. 相似文献
88.
89.
90.
Relating print and speech: the effects of letter names and word position on reading and spelling performance 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
From an early age, children can go beyond rote memorization to form links between print and speech that are based on letter names in the initial positions of words (Treiman & Rodriguez, 1999; Treiman, Sotak, & Bowman, 2001). For example, children's knowledge of the name of the letter t helps them learn that the novel word TM is pronounced as team. Four experiments were carried out to determine whether letter names at the ends of words are equally useful. Four- and five-year-olds derived little benefit from such information in reading (Experiments 1 and 3) or spelling (Experiment 2), although adults did (Experiment 4). For young children, word-final information appears to have less influence on reading and spelling performance than does word-initial information. The results help delineate the circumstances under which children can go beyond a logographic approach in learning about print. 相似文献