首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2440篇
  免费   171篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   33篇
  2022年   36篇
  2021年   36篇
  2020年   58篇
  2019年   83篇
  2018年   110篇
  2017年   108篇
  2016年   99篇
  2015年   90篇
  2014年   100篇
  2013年   270篇
  2012年   124篇
  2011年   138篇
  2010年   71篇
  2009年   91篇
  2008年   131篇
  2007年   117篇
  2006年   89篇
  2005年   80篇
  2004年   89篇
  2003年   67篇
  2002年   70篇
  2001年   36篇
  2000年   38篇
  1999年   38篇
  1998年   27篇
  1997年   27篇
  1996年   25篇
  1995年   19篇
  1994年   16篇
  1993年   15篇
  1992年   21篇
  1991年   18篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   13篇
  1987年   9篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   22篇
  1984年   13篇
  1983年   17篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   10篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   17篇
  1977年   10篇
  1976年   6篇
  1974年   13篇
  1973年   9篇
排序方式: 共有2613条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
972.
973.
Twelve low and 15 high testosterone subjects, representing the extremes in testosterone from a pool of 125 men and 128 women, kept diary records of their day-to-day thoughts and activities. Electronic pagers prompted subjects approximately 20 times over a four-day period to write down their locations, activity, thoughts, and feelings. Human coders and computer text analysis programs indicated a restless energy in the diaries of high testosterone subjects. High testosterone subjects experienced more arousal and tension than those low in testosterone. They spent more time thinking, especially about concrete problems in the immediate present. They wanted to get things done and felt frustrated when they could not. They mentioned friends more than family or lovers. Five terms characterized high testosterone subjects: restless, thinking, about action, right now, likely with friends.  相似文献   
974.
Patients with probable Alzheimer's disease (pAD) were exposed to a new verb in a naturalistic fashion. We probed their knowledge of the word's semantic and grammatical characteristics for several minutes following this exposure, and compared this with their performance on parallel measures assessing known words. Significant differences were seen between pAD patients and controls in the acquisition of the new verb's semantic meaning and its argument structure, but pAD patients did not differ from controls in the acquisition of the new word's grammatical form class. Individual patient analyses demonstrated parallel deficits restricted to the semantic meaning and argument structure of the new word and known words in several pAD patients, suggesting that a selective language impairment contributed to their word learning deficit. This pattern is consistent with an intimate relationship between semantic meaning and argument structure in semantic memory. Other pAD patients had difficulty learning about all aspects of the new word, despite good performance with known words, suggesting that compromised memory may have limited their lexical acquisition.  相似文献   
975.
976.
977.
Clinical features of depression (short attention span, lethargy, poor memory and shortened task persistence) appear to be incompatible with effective learning. This has led several investigators to suggest that depression is a significant cause of academic underachievement. The most adequate test of this hypothesis (Tesiny, Lefkowitz, & Gordon, 1980), however, found only small correlations between depression and academic achievement when neither IQ nor socioeconomic status were controlled. In the present study, three measures of childhood depression (Peer Nomination Inventory for Depression, Children's Depression Inventory, and an ad hoc teacher rating) were correlated with scores from the Peabody Individual Achievement Test and the Stanford Achievement Test. IQ was statistically controlled to provide a direct measure of underachievement and analyses were conducted separately for males, females, and the combined sample in a sample that was homogeneous for socioeconomic status. Although intercorrelations among the variables indicated that the sample was appropriate to test the hypothesis, only a few significant correlations were found. These findings suggest that if depression causes academic underachievement, it is a weak or uncommon effect.  相似文献   
978.
Attitudes toward industrial pollution, environmental-economic trade-offs, and endorsement of various strategies and activities to protect the environment were examined in a community sample. Environmental attitudes were measured using seven distinct factors organized around four cognitive themes: commercialist-expansionist-emphasizing industrial concerns even at the expense of environmental considerations; reformist-emphasizing the need and possibility of environmental quality; radical-emphasizing pessimism over the possibility of environmental quality, given the present economic and policital system; and finally, denial that the community has been harmed by pollution. Examination of the relationship between pollution attitudes and endorsement of the various proenvironmental activities yielded a pattern consistent with a cognitive, syllogistic model of the organization of attitudes. Those emphasizing a commercialist position neither favored nor regarded the various environmental activities as effective. Those who emphasized reform endorsed the various activities as both favorable and effective. Denial was associated with opposition to the various activities. Finally, those taking a radical position favored the various strategies but were not convinced of their efficacy.  相似文献   
979.
Preliminary exploration of worry: some characteristics and processes   总被引:14,自引:1,他引:13  
The present paper reports two questionnaire studies and an experimental investigation of worrying. Worry correlated more highly than general tension reports with a variety of affect scales and was characterized by: (a) feelings of anxiety, tension and apprehension; (b) moderate awareness of somatic cues including muscle tension and upset stomach; and (c) concerns over future rather than past or present situations. Self-labeled worriers were most distinguished from non-worriers by their reported uncontrollability of cognitive intrusions once worrying was initiated. In the experimental study, worriers and non-worriers were assessed for their ability to focus their attention on a monotonous (breathing) stimulus before and after 0, 15 or 30 min of worrying. Worriers reported significantly higher anxiety. depression and hostility, a lower frequency of focused attention and a greater frequency of negative thought intrusions than did non-worriers prior to the worry period. For both worriers and non-worriers. 15 min of worry resulted in an apparent incubation of negative cognitive intrusions on the subsequent attention-focusing task.  相似文献   
980.
Nairn, A., & Associates. The Reign of ETS. Today's Education, 1980, 69, 58G–64G. Kaplan, R.M. Nader's Raid on the Testing Industry: Is it in the Best Interest of the Consumer? American Psychologist, 1982, 37, 15–23.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号