全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1123篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1127篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 32篇 |
2021年 | 33篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 47篇 |
2018年 | 100篇 |
2017年 | 84篇 |
2016年 | 86篇 |
2015年 | 38篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 125篇 |
2012年 | 41篇 |
2011年 | 34篇 |
2010年 | 36篇 |
2009年 | 55篇 |
2008年 | 46篇 |
2007年 | 34篇 |
2006年 | 27篇 |
2005年 | 32篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1127条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
This study is based on exploring the trust between peers and the individual characteristics related to homophobic bullying in adolescence. Participants were 334 adolescents (141 boys and 193 girls) aged from 15 to 20 years. Participants completed the homophobic bullying scale, to investigate bullying actions towards sexual minorities, the personal attributes questionnaire, for personal characteristics, and the inventory of parent and peer attachment, to the trust between peers. The results demonstrated how female participants manifested a higher level of interpersonal traits and trust among peers than boys. Results showed how a lack of trust between peers predicts homophobic bullying. Nevertheless, we found that the lack of interpersonal characteristics represents a variable that likely mediates the relationship between lack of trust in peers and homophobic bullying. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. 相似文献
62.
63.
Janette Moore Gillian W. Smith Mark Shevlin Francis A. O’Neill 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2010,38(7):921-934
An alternative models framework was used to test three confirmatory factor analytic models for the Short Leyton Obsessional
Inventory-Children’s Version (Short LOI-CV) in a general population sample of 517 young adolescent twins (11–16 years). A
one-factor model as implicit in current classification systems of Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder (OCD), a two-factor obsessions
and compulsions model, and a multidimensional model corresponding to the three proposed subscales of the Short LOI-CV (labelled
Obsessions/Incompleteness, Numbers/Luck and Cleanliness) were considered. The three-factor model was the only model to provide
an adequate explanation of the data. Twin analyses suggested significant quantitative sex differences in heritability for
both the Obsessions/Incompleteness and Numbers/Luck dimensions with these being significantly heritable in males only (heritability
of 60% and 65% respectively). The correlation between the additive genetic effects for these two dimensions in males was 0.95
suggesting they largely share the same genetic risk factors. 相似文献
64.
Kartik K. Sreenivasan Caterina Gratton Jason Vytlacil Mark D’Esposito 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2014,14(1):117-128
Isolating the short-term storage component of working memory (WM) from the myriad of associated executive processes has been an enduring challenge. Recent efforts have identified patterns of activity in visual regions that contain information about items being held in WM. However, it remains unclear (1) whether these representations withstand intervening sensory input and (2) how communication between multimodal association cortex and the unimodal perceptual regions supporting WM representations is involved in WM storage. We present evidence that the features of a face held in WM are stored within face-processing regions, that these representations persist across subsequent sensory input, and that information about the match between sensory input and a memory representation is relayed forward from perceptual to prefrontal regions. Participants were presented with a series of probe faces and indicated whether each probe matched a target face held in WM. We parametrically varied the feature similarity between the probe and target faces. Activity within face-processing regions scaled linearly with the degree of feature similarity between the probe face and the features of the target face, suggesting that the features of the target face were stored in these regions. Furthermore, directed connectivity measures revealed that the direction of information flow that was optimal for performance was from sensory regions that stored the features of the target face to dorsal prefrontal regions, supporting the notion that sensory input is compared to representations stored within perceptual regions and is subsequently relayed forward. Together, these findings indicate that WM storage operations are carried out within perceptual cortex. 相似文献
65.
Massimo Dell’Utri 《Journal for General Philosophy of Science》2017,48(4):501-515
Two convictions underlie the following article. The first is that Hilary Putnam has been one of the greatest thinkers of our time, a philosopher who was able to propose groundbreaking ideas in virtually every area of philosophy. As the reader will see, the topics he tackled in his writings included questions of philosophy of science, philosophy of language, philosophy of mathematics and logic, philosophy of mind, metaethics, the fact-value dichotomy, the interpretation of Wittgenstein’s later thought, the question of relativism, the analysis of rationality, the analysis of religious experience, the character of Jewish philosophy, the interpretation of pragmatism, the elucidation of the concept of truth, the question of realism, the relationship between mind and the world. The second is that the changes some of his positions underwent, far from being a point of weakness—as some critics have sometimes felt compelled to claim—reveal the freshness and genuineness of Putnam’s way of philosophising and at the same time the essence of philosophical discussion itself. 相似文献
66.
Casey O’Callaghan 《Philosophical Studies》2011,153(1):143-160
Recent work on non-visual modalities aims to translate, extend, revise, or unify claims about perception beyond vision. This
paper presents central lessons drawn from attention to hearing, sounds, and multimodality. It focuses on auditory awareness
and its objects, and it advances more general lessons for perceptual theorizing that emerge from thinking about sounds and
audition. The paper argues that sounds and audition no better support the privacy of perception’s objects than does vision;
that perceptual objects are more diverse than an exclusively visual perspective suggests; and that multimodality is rampant.
In doing so, it presents an account according to which audition affords awareness as of not just sounds, but also environmental
happenings beyond sounds. 相似文献
67.
68.
Vasco d’Agnese 《Studies in Philosophy and Education》2016,35(2):195-214
In this article I argue that Dewey, throughout his work, conducted a systematic dismantling of the concept of rationality as mastery and control. Such a dismantling entails, at the same time, the dismantling of the auto-grounded subject, namely, the subject that grounds itself in the power to master experience. The Deweyan challenge to Western ontology goes straight to the core of the subject’s question. Dewey not only systematically challenged the understanding of thinking as a process consciously managed by the subject but also conceived of thinking as an event rather than a process—something that occurs in us rather than something intentionally staged by a reflective subject. Such a twofold dismantling of rationality and subject rather than a flow in a nihilistic/relativistic account of education results in a reinforcement of education that must be understood not so much as the attempt to understand and predict experience but as the means to create new, unpredictable experience. As a result, education, for Dewey, is grounded on, moved by, and directed at uncertainty. Education, in a sense, engenders uncertainty. 相似文献
69.
70.