全文获取类型
收费全文 | 23300篇 |
免费 | 787篇 |
国内免费 | 21篇 |
专业分类
24108篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 218篇 |
2019年 | 230篇 |
2018年 | 331篇 |
2017年 | 385篇 |
2016年 | 375篇 |
2015年 | 244篇 |
2014年 | 317篇 |
2013年 | 1757篇 |
2012年 | 629篇 |
2011年 | 689篇 |
2010年 | 425篇 |
2009年 | 393篇 |
2008年 | 582篇 |
2007年 | 583篇 |
2006年 | 524篇 |
2005年 | 474篇 |
2004年 | 474篇 |
2003年 | 434篇 |
2002年 | 496篇 |
2001年 | 735篇 |
2000年 | 751篇 |
1999年 | 570篇 |
1998年 | 273篇 |
1997年 | 224篇 |
1992年 | 465篇 |
1991年 | 422篇 |
1990年 | 458篇 |
1989年 | 424篇 |
1988年 | 412篇 |
1987年 | 408篇 |
1986年 | 417篇 |
1985年 | 430篇 |
1984年 | 350篇 |
1983年 | 310篇 |
1982年 | 245篇 |
1981年 | 230篇 |
1979年 | 402篇 |
1978年 | 289篇 |
1977年 | 260篇 |
1976年 | 234篇 |
1975年 | 348篇 |
1974年 | 382篇 |
1973年 | 362篇 |
1972年 | 331篇 |
1971年 | 293篇 |
1970年 | 265篇 |
1969年 | 324篇 |
1968年 | 363篇 |
1967年 | 330篇 |
1966年 | 330篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
51.
J T Cacioppo J S Martzke R E Petty L G Tassinary 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,54(4):592-604
Previous research has demonstrated that mild negative emotional imagery and unpleasant sensory stimuli lead to greater electromyographic activity over the brow muscle region than mild positive imagery and stimuli, even in the absence of significant changes in visceral and general facial EMG activity. Previous research has not addressed whether electromyographic responses over the brow region are a sensitive and specific index of emotions, however, since a multiplicity of events lead to changes in brow activity. In this research, facial electromyographic and audiovisual recordings were obtained while individuals were interviewed about themselves. Afterwards, individuals were asked to describe what they had been thinking of during specific segments of the interview marked by distinctive electromyographic responses over the brow region in the context of ongoing but stable levels of activity elsewhere in the face. The results are interpreted in terms of a continuous flow hypothesis of affect-laden information processing. 相似文献
52.
Perceptual sensitivity to dimensional and similarity relations in free and rule-governed classifications 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Two experiments were conducted to investigate the perceptual primacy of dimensional and similarity relations in the stimulus classifications of younger and older subjects. In Experiment 1, 4- and 10-year-olds were given free classifications in which they could group stimuli according to overall similarity or identities in size, color, or orientation. Both age groups classified stimuli most frequently according to identities on separate dimensions. In Experiment 2, 4-year-olds and adults were given free classifications followed by rule-governed classifications which required them to group stimuli according to specific relations. In the free classifications, a majority of subjects in both age groups classified the stimuli most frequently according to identities on separate dimensions. In the rule-governed classifications, both age groups were more accurate when a single separate relation was required for solution than when overall similarity was required. These results support a differential-sensitivity view of perceptual development, which asserts that individuals at all ages primarily perceive and use separate relations. 相似文献
53.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
In recent years in research on intergroup relationships, the assumption has increasingly been made that discrimination dominates decisions when individuals allocate resources between (members of) own and other group. Conversely, in empirical studies of interpersonal decision-making, including an extensive literature on the development of children's allocation rules within dyadic relationships, it has been repeatedly observed that in dyadic relationships choices though responsive to various changes in the environment, are more strongly governed by fairness rules. The present research extends the interpersonal fairness paradigm to the intergroup case, and examines the effects of some of those variables, namely, children's age, input and attitudes toward other, that have been observed to influence choice behaviour within interpersonal relationships. The findings indicate that as children are socialized, fairness rules also play an increasing dominant role in intergroup allocation decisions, and that both relative input and the language of the outgroup influence such decisions. At the same time, there is some preliminary evidence to indicate that the relative strength of self-interest may be somewhat stronger in intergroup than in interpersonal relationships. Finally, a number of the issues that must be confronted in comparing the two more important forms of human social choices, interpersonal and intergroup decision-making, are considered. 相似文献
59.
Cyd C. Strauss Cynthia G. Last Michel Hersen Alan E. Kazdin 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1988,16(1):57-68
The relationship between anxiety and depression was examined in a sample of 106 children and adolescents referred to an outpatient anxiety disorder clinic for children. Twenty-eight percent of patients with DSM-III diagnoses of anxiety disorders displayed a concurrent major depression. Children with anxiety disorders plus major depression were found (1) to be older, (2) to demonstrate more severe anxiety symptomatology, and (3) to be diagnosed with different rates of certain anxiety-disorder subtypes, when compared to anxious patients without major depression. Nondepressed anxious children and adolescents did not differ from a psychopathological control group in severity of either anxiety or depression symptoms. 相似文献
60.
Cyd C. Strauss Cynthia A. Lease Cynthia G. Last Greta Francis 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1988,16(4):433-443
Differences between a clinical sample of younger (ages 5 to 11) and older (ages 12 to 19) children meeting DSM-III criteria for overanxious disorder (OAD) were examined. Younger and older children were compared in terms of (1) the rates of OAD diagnoses occurring in the two age groups, (2) sociodemographic characteristics, (3) symptom expression, (4) association with other forms of maladjustment, and (5) self-reported anxiety and depression. The prevalence of OAD diagnoses and sociodemographic characteristics did not differ. Although younger and older OAD children showed similar rates of most specific DSM-III OAD symptoms, older children presented with a higher total number of overanxious symptoms than younger children. Older children more frequently exhibited a concurrent major depression or simple phobia, whereas younger OAD children more commonly had coexisting separation anxiety or attention deficit disorders. Older OAD children reported significantly higher levels of anxiety and depression on self-report measures. Findings indicated that the expression of OAD varies by developmental level. 相似文献