全文获取类型
收费全文 | 581篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
589篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 18篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 66篇 |
2012年 | 27篇 |
2011年 | 15篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 22篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 13篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 7篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有589条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
Three experiments tested preschoolers' use of abstract principles to classify and label objects by shape or function. Three- and 4-year-olds were instructed to match objects by shape or function. Four-year-olds readily adopted either rule, but 3-year-olds followed only the shape rule. Without a rule, 4-year-olds tended to match by shape unless object function was shown during matching (Experiment 2). Three-year-olds' ability to use a function rule was tested in several conditions (re-presenting functions; reminders to "use the rule"; repeating rule on every trial). None induced consistent function matching (Experiment 3). Supplemental memory and verbal tasks showed that 3-year-olds have trouble using function as an abstract basis of comparison. Naming data, however, show that preschoolers are learning that object labels are based on function. The results show preschoolers' growing flexibility in adopting abstract generalization rules and growing knowledge of conventions for extending words. 相似文献
182.
This paper poses a feminist challenge to stage models of spiritual development and suggests alternative metaphors to the common image of a heroic journey. Women's spirituality is typically more intuitive and relational than rational and individualistic and to express this, we employ metaphors of the web and the quilt. Support for the relevance of these metaphors comes from the spiritual autobiography of Jane Goodall, an anthology of African American women's writings about spirituality, and two qualitative research investigations of older women's spirituality. We conclude with an autobiographical account of our own spiritual development and engage in a dialogue-on-the-page in which we respond to each other's narratives. 相似文献
183.
In this study, the authors examined differences between Chinese and American commercial arbitrators. They predicted and found that Chinese arbitrators make higher awards for interfirm contract violations than Americans. This difference is partially explained by differences in attributions. Prior theory suggests, and the authors found, that the Chinese tend to have more internal attributions for events when observing group actions. When evidence provided to arbitrators is mixed (evidence is provided for both internal and external attributions), Chinese-American differences in awards become even stronger. 相似文献
184.
185.
186.
Grégoire Vitry Nathalie Duriez Sophie Lartilleux-Suberville Rytis Pakrosnis Alexandrina Beau Teresa Garcia-Rivera Olivier Brosseau Pedro Vargas Avalos Eric Bardot Wendel A. Ray 《Family process》2020,59(4):1946-1957
SYPRENE, a new international Systemic Practice Research Network (PRN), has been established to fill the gap in practice-based research on the effectiveness and efficiency of strategic therapies. This article presents the rationale for the creation of SYPRENE and describes data collection methods, and the encoding system implemented within this PRN. More developments are expected in the recruitment of practitioners, the types of data collected, findings, and the implementation of SYPRENE in supervision, trainings, and professional schools. 相似文献
187.
Ray Ison 《World Futures: Journal of General Evolution》2019,75(1-2):5-16
This article is an invitation to be reflexive; reflexivity is a second-order process or reflection on reflection. The possibility that a reader might experience a reflexive moment is sought by avoiding a narrative trap: to believe the “coming to” of the issue title implies a state to arrive at, carefully planned, a purposeful journey, pursued by an enlightened individual devoid of all social relations. The author thus begins situated in a social system. Following Maturana, a social system is explained, as is what constitutes, or triggers, change, in a social system. An example of granting rivers sentience in law as an expansion of the social is explored 相似文献
188.
Jaime Kucinskas Bradley R. E. Wright D. Matthew Ray John Ortberg 《Journal for the scientific study of religion》2017,56(2):418-437
We explore how people experience the sacred in their everyday lives using a recently developed research technique—smartphone‐based experience sampling method (S‐ESM). The primary goal of our experience‐driven approach is to explore the contours and variations of spiritual awareness within people's day‐to‐day lives. We seek to better understand when and where spiritual awareness is likely to arise, and the contexts in which it is rare. Our smartphone‐based data allow us to track the many contexts in which an awareness of the sacred occurs, as reported in real time during people's normal daily activities. We parse out how immediate contextual factors and how people's more habitual behaviors are related to their spiritual experiences. This illuminates a wide range of factors that influence spiritual experiences that have not received much scholarly attention, and enables us to connect cutting‐edge quantitative methods with qualitative scholarship on spirituality. We hope this will open the door to the development of new theories of situated spiritual experience. 相似文献
189.
Ray Buchanan 《Thought: A Journal of Philosophy》2012,1(2):152-157
Grice's (1957) analysis of non‐natural meaning generated a huge industry, where new analyses were put forward to respond to successively more complex counterexamples. Davis (2003) offers a novel and refreshingly simple analysis of meaning in terms of the expression of belief, where (roughly) an agent expresses the belief that p just in case she performs a publicly observable action with the intention that it be an indication that she occurrently believes that p. I argue that Davis's analysis fails to capture the essentially overt nature of our meaning‐intentions, and with it, a plausible sufficient condition for meaning. 相似文献
190.
A large body of research now exists which demonstrates that the cognitive interview (CI) increases the reporting of correct recall from various population groups. It is now necessary to determine from both a theoretical and an applied perspective which of the CI techniques, or combination of techniques are largely responsible for this CI superiority effect. The two aims of this study were (1) to examine the relative effectiveness of each of the four original CI mnemonics and (2) to assess their efficacy as a function of age. A total of 125 participants; 34 first‐year undergraduate students, 44 children aged 8–9 years and 47 children aged 5–6 years viewed a video‐recording of an accident. Forty‐eight hours later each participant was individually interviewed and randomly assigned to one of six instruction groups; (1) context reinstatement, (2) change perspective, (3) change order, (4) report everything, (5) report everything + context reinstatement combination (RE + CR), or (6) a control instruction to ‘try again’. Each of the individual CI mnemonics were found to be of equal benefit and to occasion no more recall than the ‘try again’ control. However, the RE + CR combination resulted in significantly more correct recall compared to the individual mnemonics. There was no effect of age group on the efficacy of the various instructions. Copyright © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献