全文获取类型
收费全文 | 573篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
581篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 18篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 69篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 19篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 7篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有581条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
401.
The Revised Transliminality Scale and Zuckerman's Sensation Seeking Scale (Form V) were administered to 83 psychology undergraduates. Weak positive correlations suggested that participants scoring high on the Transliminality Scale tended to be more experience seeking and, overall, more inclined to score sensation seeking. 相似文献
402.
Nicolosi and Byrd in 2002 summarized empirical research on birth order and sexual orientation in men, which research has documented that homosexual men have a later birth order than heterosexual men. They did not, however, note a more refined analysis of an earlier null finding by Siegelman. This 1998 reanalysis by Blanchard, Zucker, Siegelman, Dickey, and Klassen also confirmed the later birth order of homosexual men. 相似文献
403.
Groups in conflict develop strikingly different construals of the same violent events. These clashing perceptions of past violence can have detrimental consequences for intergroup relations and might provoke new hostilities. In this article, we integrate and juxtapose what we know about construals of collective violence by delineating the different dimensions along which these construals differ between victim and perpetrator groups: regarding the question of who is the victim, who is responsible for the harm doing, what the perpetrator’s intent was, how severe the violence was, and when it took place. Then, we discuss the individual‐ and group‐level factors (e.g., collective narratives, social identities) that shape these construals, as well as their implications for attitudes regarding the conflict and support for relevant policies. We distinguish two different core motives that drive construals and their outcomes among victim and perpetrator groups: Perpetrator groups try to cope with moral identity threats and preserve a positive image of the ingroup, while victim groups try to protect their ingroup from future harm doing and desire acknowledgment of their group’s experiences. Lastly, we discuss implications for strategies and interventions to address victim and perpetrator groups’ divergent perspectives of collective violence. 相似文献
404.
Sieun An Navya Kapoor Aanchal Setia Smriti Agiwal Ishika Ray 《Journal of Beliefs & Values》2019,40(2):205-214
Despite humans’ capacity for rational thought, they are not immune to superstitions. Superstitions are strongly tied to cultural practices, especially in India. Although 17% of the world’s population resides in India, Indian culture is understudied, and there have not been sufficient attempts to understand Indian superstitions in a scientific manner from a psychometric standpoint. By creating a proper superstition measurement for the Indian population, we can better understand how Indians think and behave. The goal of the present research is to create a superstition measure specific to Indian culture. The results reveal 18 items reflecting Indian superstitions that can be generalised across contemporary India. 相似文献
405.
Despite the abundance of evidence demonstrating a dedicated link between positive and negative affect and specific ways of thinking, not all findings are consistent with this view. New research suggests that the relationship between affect and thinking can be altered and often reversed, by varying the mental context in which affect is experienced. The affect‐as‐cognitive‐feedback account can explain a wide range of phenomena, including both prior findings and this more recent research, and generate new predictions. According to this account, affective reactions confer value on accessible information processing strategies (e.g., global vs. local processing) and other mental content that happens to be accessible at the time. This view underscores that the relationship between affect and cognition is not fixed but instead is highly malleable. We present evidence that supports this account, along with new findings that suggest a malleable influence of specific affective states on cognition. 相似文献
406.
Cindy Brooks Dollar Bradley Ray Mary Kay Hudson Brittany J. Hood 《Behavioral sciences & the law》2018,36(1):32-45
The number of problem‐solving courts has grown substantially since the mid‐1990s. Research consistently indicates that participation in these courts lowers recidivism, which is often attributed to defendants’ increased perceptions of procedural justice in these programs. Yet, prior studies are limited in their focus, often examining interactions with the judge in a single court or examining defendant perceptions and outcomes at a single time point. In the present study, we investigate defendant perceptions of procedural justice with judges and case managers across multiple problem‐solving courts over time. Findings indicate that procedural justice varies across court actors and over time. Procedural justice is lower among judges than among case managers; however, changes in perceptions of procedural justice with the judge are associated with improved court outcomes. We suggest that defendant perceptions are variable and complex but important in explaining variations in outcomes. 相似文献
407.
The present study sought to expand the literature on the relations of major dimensions of personality with integrative negotiation outcomes by introducing the HEXACO model, investigating both effects of the negotiators' and their counterparts' personality traits on objective and subjective negotiation outcomes, and investigating two interactions between the negotiators' and counterparts' personalities. One hundred forty‐eight participants completed the HEXACO‐100 measure of personality. Participants then engaged in a dyadic negotiation task that contained a mix of distributive and integrative elements (74 dyads). Measures of subjective experience and objective economic value were obtained, and actor–partner interdependence models were estimated. Personality was generally a better predictor of subjective experience than objective economic value. In particular, partner honesty‐humility, extraversion, and openness predicted more positive negotiation experiences. An actor–partner interaction effect was found for actor‐agreeableness by partner‐honesty‐humility on economic outcomes; agreeable actors achieved worse (better) economic outcomes when negotiating with partners that were low (high) on honesty‐humility. © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology 相似文献
408.
409.
410.