全文获取类型
收费全文 | 640篇 |
免费 | 45篇 |
专业分类
685篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 15篇 |
2018年 | 18篇 |
2017年 | 13篇 |
2016年 | 29篇 |
2015年 | 16篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 60篇 |
2012年 | 26篇 |
2011年 | 48篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 29篇 |
2007年 | 32篇 |
2006年 | 21篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有685条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
42.
Cooper ML Wood PK Orcutt HK Albino A 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2003,84(2):390-410
Longitudinal data from a representative sample of 1,978 Black and White adolescents were used to examine the role of personality in multiple risk or problem behaviors. Results indicate that covariation among diverse behaviors (educational underachievement, delinquent behavior, substance use, sexual behavior) can be adequately modeled by a single higher order factor, and that impulsivity and avoidance coping serve as generalized risk factors for involvement in these behaviors. Whereas none of the personality variables examined explained change in problem behaviors once established, avoidance coping prospectively predicted involvement among a subgroup of adolescents with little or no prior involvement. Results suggest that dysfunctional styles of regulating emotions and emotionally driven behaviors are core features of risky or problem behaviors during adolescence. 相似文献
43.
International Journal for Philosophy of Religion - 相似文献
44.
45.
Tacit Skills, the Japanese Management Model and New Technology 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Stephen Wood 《Psychologie appliquee》1990,39(2):169-190
46.
A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The present model outlines the mechanisms underlying habitual control of responding and the ways in which habits interface with goals. Habits emerge from the gradual learning of associations between responses and the features of performance contexts that have historically covaried with them (e.g., physical settings, preceding actions). Once a habit is formed, perception of contexts triggers the associated response without a mediating goal. Nonetheless, habits interface with goals. Constraining this interface, habit associations accrue slowly and do not shift appreciably with current goal states or infrequent counterhabitual responses. Given these constraints, goals can (a) direct habits by motivating repetition that leads to habit formation and by promoting exposure to cues that trigger habits, (b) be inferred from habits, and (c) interact with habits in ways that preserve the learned habit associations. Finally, the authors outline the implications of the model for habit change, especially for the self-regulation of habit cuing. 相似文献
47.
Sher KJ Bartholow BD Peuser K Erickson DJ Wood MD 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2007,116(2):362-377
The present study sought to characterize alcohol's stress-response-dampening (SRD) effects on multiple measures of stress and whether these effects are mediated by reductions in sustained attention and, further, whether baseline levels of sustained attention moderate SRD. One hundred six men consumed either an alcohol (0.70 g/kg) or a placebo beverage prior to learning that they would deliver a self-disclosing speech. Structural equation models controlling for multiple baseline periods indicated that alcohol directly reduced self-reported anxiety and skin conductance levels in response to the stressor. Alcohol's effect on reducing heart rate response, in contrast, was indirect and mediated by effects on prestress baseline. As hypothesized, differences in sustained attention partially mediated the effects of alcohol on skin conductance (but not heart rate or self-reported anxiety) and served as a moderator of alcohol's effects on skin conductance response. Findings are discussed in terms of theoretical links among alcohol consumption, specific cognitive abilities, and stress reactivity. 相似文献
48.
Separate verbal memory and naming deficits in attention deficit disorder and reading disability 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
In this study, verbal memory and naming abilities were investigated in reading disabled (RD) and control children who were characterized according to the presence or absence of attention deficit disorder (ADD). Results indicate that deficits in learning and memory for recently acquired information occur as a function of ADD rather than RD while deficits in naming are specific to RD rather than ADD. We conclude that ADD is a major source of additional and separate cognitive morbidity in RD children. 相似文献
49.
Richard Schulz Connie A. Tompkins Dan Wood Susan Decker 《Journal of applied social psychology》1987,17(4):401-428
The emphasis of much of the existing research on social support-health relationships has been on the availability and impact of support on the recipient, but social psychologists have paid little attention to the costs to the caregiver of providing that support. In an earlier study (Schulz & Decker, 1985), we found that the long-term coping of individuals experiencing chronic disability as a result of spinal-cord injury was facilitated by the availability of a primary support person, or caregiver. In this study we examined the determinants of well-being of the primary caregiver of the middle-aged and elderly spinal-cord-injured persons. Psychological well-being, life satisfaction, and depression were the dependent measures. The availability of social support, social contact, and feelings of control over one's life were important determinants of caregiver well-being, even after controlling for health and income. In addition, the amount of time spent each day assisting the disabled person was a strong independent correlate of depression, and the extent to which the caregiver felt burdened by the victim was significantly and negatively related to life satisfaction. These results are discussed in the context of a comprehensive stress-coping model. Since data were available from both disabled individuals and their primary support persons, we had the unique opportunity to compare the perspectives of both those giving and receiving daily care. We found no relationship in levels of psychological well-being and depression reported by the disabled person and caregiver; however, caregivers attributed to the victims affective levels similar to their own. 相似文献
50.
Studies 1 and 2 addressed people's perceptions of women's and men's overall maladaptive worry. In Study 1, participants rated how they perceive women's and men's worry in general. In Study 2, participants rated the worry of either a woman or a man whom they know well. As expected in both studies, participants perceived women as experiencing more maladaptive worry than men. A second objective was to compare people's perceptions of women's and men's worry to their perceptions of low- and high-status individuals' worry to determine if a status model can account for the perception that women worry more than men. In Study 3, participants were presented a minimal instantiation of status (Conway, Pizzamiglio, & Mount, 1996). As expected, participants perceived low-status individuals as experiencing more maladaptive worry than high-status individuals. Findings are discussed in terms of gender stereotypes and theoretical models of gender. 相似文献