全文获取类型
收费全文 | 998篇 |
免费 | 13篇 |
专业分类
1011篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 19篇 |
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 38篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 60篇 |
2017年 | 50篇 |
2016年 | 39篇 |
2015年 | 33篇 |
2014年 | 30篇 |
2013年 | 108篇 |
2012年 | 84篇 |
2011年 | 86篇 |
2010年 | 59篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 60篇 |
2007年 | 63篇 |
2006年 | 59篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1011条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
251.
Robert Sternfeld Graeme Forbes Ronald M. Green Lorenzo Peña Manuel Liz Mark Rowlands 《Philosophia》1994,24(1-2):225-252
252.
Héctor-Neri Castañeda 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》1972,1(3-4):467-480
Conclusion I am pleased to have been able to vindicate Plato from the oft-rehearsed charge of not having distinguished relations from qualities. Not only does Phaedo 102B7-C4 show quite clearly that he did make the proper distinction, but the theory of relations he adumbrated there is logically sound and ontologically viable. Furthermore, it is refreshing to think of relations not as Forms or universals, but as chains of ontologically tied universals.Naturally, now that we have a clear understanding of Plato's Phaedo theory of relations and relational facts there is plenty of work to do. We must examine the other dialogues for alterations or even preservation of that theory. Moreover, there are those arguments of Aristotle that purport to reduce Plato's Theory of Forms to absurdity on account of relations. But of this I shall say more at some other time. 相似文献
253.
People can generate the same thoughts or process the same information with different degrees of ease, and this subjective experience has implications for attitudes and social judgment. In prior research, it has generally been assumed that the experience of ease or fluency is interpreted by people as something good. In the two experiments reported here, the meaning or value of ease was directly manipulated, and the implications for evaluative judgments were explored. Across experiments, we replicated the traditional ease-of-retrieval effect (more thought-congruent attitudes when thoughts were easy rather than difficult to generate) when ease was described as positive, but we reversed this effect when ease was described as negative. These findings suggest that it is important to consider both the content of metacognition (e.g., "those thoughts were easy to generate") and the value associated with that content (e.g., "ease is good" or "ease is bad"). 相似文献
254.
Jiménez L Vaquero JM Lupiáñez J 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2006,32(3):475-490
Four experiments investigate the differences between implicit and explicit sequence learning concerning their resilience to structural and superficial task changes. A superficial change that embedded the SRT task in the context of a selection task, while maintaining the sequence, did selectively hinder the expression of implicit learning. In contrast, a manipulation that maintained the task surface, but decreased the sequence validity, affected the expression of learning specifically when it was explicit. These results are discussed in the context of a dynamic framework (Cleeremans & Jiménez, 2002), which assumes that implicit knowledge is specially affected by contextual factors and that, as knowledge becomes explicit, it allows for the development of relevant metaknowledge that modulates the expression of explicit knowledge. 相似文献
255.
The present study evaluates the extent to which central categories explicitly labeled as being in the middle of the other response categories, specifically Middle Level and Sometimes, function as expected according to the integer scoring system. The assumptions are tested by means of Bock's Nominal Model in two 5-response scales. Results show that the assumption of the ordering of the response categories is met for all the items. The ordering of thresholds is satisfied for all but one item with the central category Middle Level . Results are compared with those obtained when middle categories are not explicitly labeled as being in the middle of the other response categories, as in the case of Not Sure , Undecided or? 相似文献
256.
257.
Two studies were conducted in which human participants rated pairs of words according to the perceived degree to which the
words’ referents shared semantic features. The participants found the task intuitive, simple, and quick to complete. The ratings
were reliable and valid. Interrater and interstudy correlations were high, and ratings were good predictors of known feature
overlap values obtained from existing semantic feature norms. 相似文献
258.
There is a growing body of evidence showing that a word's cognate status is an important dimension affecting the naming performance of bilingual speakers. In a recent article, Kohnert extended this observation to the naming performance of an aphasic bilingual (DJ). DJ named pictures with cognate names more accurately than pictures with non-cognate names. Furthermore, having named the pictures in Spanish helped the subsequent retrieval (with a delay of one week between the two tests) of the same pictures' names in English, but only for pictures with cognate names. That is, there was a language transfer but only for those translation words that were phonologically similar. In this article we first evaluate the conclusions drawn from these results by Kohnert, and second we discuss the theoretical implications of the facilitatory effects of cognate words for models of speech production in bilingual speakers. 相似文献
259.
Beckers T De Houwer J Pineño O Miller RR 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2005,31(2):238-249
Recent research suggests that outcome additivity pretraining modulates blocking in human causal learning. However, the existing evidence confounds outcome additivity and outcome maximality. Here the authors present evidence for the influence of presenting information about outcome maximality (Experiment 1) and outcome additivity (Experiment 2) on subsequent forward blocking. The results of Experiment 3 confirm that, with outcome maximality controlled, outcome additivity affects backward blocking but not release from overshadowing. Finally, the results of Experiment 4 demonstrate that information about outcome additivity has a similar effect on forward blocking if presented after the blocking training instead of before. The results are compatible with the idea that blocking results from inferential processes at the time of testing and not from a failure to acquire associative strength during training. 相似文献
260.