全文获取类型
收费全文 | 550篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
562篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 27篇 |
2016年 | 34篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 76篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 33篇 |
2010年 | 19篇 |
2009年 | 22篇 |
2008年 | 38篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 21篇 |
2003年 | 17篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有562条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
212.
213.
Abraham L Halpern John H Halpern Sean B Doherty 《Philosophy, ethics, and humanities in medicine : PEHM》2008,3(1):21
After revelations of participation by psychiatrists and psychologists in interrogation of prisoners at Guantánamo Bay and
Central Intelligence Agency secret detention centers, the American Psychiatric Association and the American Psychological
Association adopted Position Statements absolutely prohibiting their members from participating in torture under any and all
circumstances, and, to a limited degree, forbidding involvement in interrogations. Some interrogations utilize very aggressive
techniques determined to be torture by many nations and organizations throughout the world. This paper explains why psychiatrists
and psychologists involved in coercive interrogations violate the Geneva Conventions and the laws of the United States. Whether
done with ignorance of professional ethical obligations or not, these psychiatrists and psychologists have crossed an ethical
barrier that may best be averted from re-occurring by teaching medical students and residents in all medical specialties about
the ethics principles stemming from the 1946–1947 Nuremberg trials and the Geneva Conventions, together with the Ethics Codes
of the World Medical Association and the American Medical Association; and, with regard to psychiatric residents and psychological
trainees, by the teaching about The Principles of Medical Ethics With Annotations Especially Applicable to Psychiatry and the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct, respectively. In this way, all physicians and psychologists will clearly understand that they have an absolute moral obligation
to "First, do no harm" to the human beings they professionally encounter. 相似文献
214.
The age-appropriate Sport Anxiety Scale-2 (SAS-2; Smith, Smoll, Cumming, & Grossbard, 2006) was used to assess levels of cognitive and somatic anxiety among male and female youth sport participants. Confirmatory factor analyses with a sample of 9-14 year old athletes (N=1038) supported the viability of a three-factor model of anxiety involving somatic anxiety, worry, and concentration disruption previously demonstrated in high school and college samples. Tests for factorial invariance revealed that the three-factor model was an equally good fit for 9-11 year olds and 12-14 year olds, and for both males and females. Gender and age were modestly related to anxiety scores. Worry about performing poorly was highest in girls and in older athletes, whereas boys reported higher levels of concentration disruption in competitive sport situations. Implications for emotional perception and for the study of competitive anxiety in young athletes are discussed. 相似文献
215.
Construal level theory suggests that events and objects can be represented at either a higher, more abstract level involving consideration of superordinate goals, desirability, global processing, and broad categorizations or a lower, more concrete level involving consideration of subordinate goals, feasibility, local processing, and narrow categorizations. Analogously, social targets (including the self) can be represented more broadly, as members of a group, or more narrowly, as individuals. Because abstract construals induce a similarity focus, they were predicted to increase the perceived fit between social targets and a salient social category. Accordingly, placing individuals into a more abstract construal mind-set via an unrelated task increased the activation and use of stereotypes of salient social groups, stereotype-consistent trait ratings of the self, group identification, and stereotype-consistent performance relative to more concrete construal mind-sets. Thus, nonsocial contextual influences (construal level mind-sets) affect stereotyping of self and others. 相似文献
216.
An ideal empathizer may attend to another person's behavior in order to understand that person, but it is also possible that accurately understanding other people involves top-down strategies. We hypothesized that perceivers draw on stereotypes to infer other people's thoughts and that stereotype use increases perceivers' accuracy. In this study, perceivers (N = 161) inferred the thoughts of multiple targets. Inferences consistent with stereotypes for the targets' group (new mothers) more accurately captured targets' thoughts, particularly when actual thought content was also stereotypic. We also decomposed variance in empathic accuracy into thought, target, and perceiver variance. Although past research has frequently focused on variance between perceivers or targets (which assumes individual differences in the ability to understand other people or be understood, respectively), the current study showed that the most substantial variance was found within targets because of differences among thoughts. 相似文献
217.
Three experiments provide evidence for a primacy effect in judgments of spatial location. Participants viewed and immediately
estimated a series of spatial locations that were serially ordered from left to right or from right to left. In a subsequent
block, they judged the rightmost, leftmost, and center of the distribution or were shown dots at those locations, which they
then estimated from memory. Both judgments and memories were biased toward locations that had been presented earliest in the
sequence. The findings indicate that participants incorporate not only geometric categories, but also aspects of their prior
spatial experience, when estimating locations. The results mirror recent evidence for a primacy effect in nonspatial category
induction, suggesting that this effect generalizes across domains. 相似文献
218.
Although empathic accuracy is considered a stable skill, few individual difference measures consistently predict performance on Ickes’ (e.g., 2001) empathic accuracy measure. Because past work has shown that women are more empathically accurate than men when female gender roles are made salient before an empathic accuracy task, we hypothesized that self-reported communion and related variables might predict empathic accuracy. Participants (194 undergraduates) from a northwestern U.S. university completed an empathic accuracy task and self-report measures of communion and empathy. Communion and empathic concern predicted greater empathic accuracy, but only after controlling for socially desirable responding. The role of communion in empathic inference is discussed, along with the need to include measures of social desirability when examining correlates of empathic accuracy. 相似文献
219.
A multiple-baseline design was used to examine the influence of an imagery intervention on the performance of swimmers’ times on a thousand-yard practice set. Performance times for four swimmers were collected over a 15-week period during preseason training. The intervention took place over a 3-week period and was introduced after the fourth week of the study. The results revealed that three out of four participants significantly improved their times on the one thousand-yard practice set after being introduced to the imagery intervention. The results are discussed in terms of the implications of using imagery to improve athlete's performance on continuous tasks. 相似文献
220.
This research explores the relationships between each of four "domains" of family life and the health of husbands and wives in a community-based sample of 225 families. In this article we report the association between Family Structure/Organization and adult Health. This family domain refers to the architecture of the family or the structural frame of roles and rules within which the family operates. Interrelationships among 13 self-reported, family Structure/Organization scales are described, using principle components analysis (PCA) and multidimensional scaling analysis (MDS). Derived, joint-spouse or couple Structure/Organization variables also were created using inter-battery factor analysis. The PCA yielded a poor solution, whereas the MDS yielded a good two-dimensional solution, which roughly displayed the scales in a circular pattern for both husbands and wives. The analyses indicated that no single dimension or set of separate subdimensions adequately described the Structure/Organization variables. All 13 scales than were associated with a battery of 14 adult health scales for husbands and wives separately, using canonical correlation. Different aspects of family Structure/Organization were correlated with health for husbands and wives: Organized Cohesiveness, Sex Role Traditionalism, Role Flexibility and Shared Roles for husbands; and Organized Cohesiveness and Differentiated Sharing for wives. Different patterns of health scores also emerged by gender, with behavioral indicators, such as Smoking and Drinking, more salient for husbands, and mood indicators, such as Anxiety and Depression, more salient for wives. 相似文献