全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5334篇 |
免费 | 62篇 |
专业分类
5396篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 56篇 |
2023年 | 58篇 |
2022年 | 77篇 |
2021年 | 115篇 |
2020年 | 125篇 |
2019年 | 164篇 |
2018年 | 209篇 |
2017年 | 213篇 |
2016年 | 192篇 |
2015年 | 140篇 |
2014年 | 147篇 |
2013年 | 597篇 |
2012年 | 330篇 |
2011年 | 321篇 |
2010年 | 228篇 |
2009年 | 150篇 |
2008年 | 275篇 |
2007年 | 246篇 |
2006年 | 207篇 |
2005年 | 168篇 |
2004年 | 147篇 |
2003年 | 124篇 |
2002年 | 126篇 |
2001年 | 63篇 |
2000年 | 67篇 |
1999年 | 60篇 |
1998年 | 48篇 |
1997年 | 43篇 |
1996年 | 34篇 |
1995年 | 38篇 |
1994年 | 36篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 35篇 |
1991年 | 29篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 33篇 |
1988年 | 30篇 |
1987年 | 26篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 28篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 23篇 |
1982年 | 28篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 17篇 |
1976年 | 16篇 |
1973年 | 26篇 |
排序方式: 共有5396条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
Switch cost does not disappear as more preparation time for the next task is allowed. Tornay and Milán showed that the residual cost is smaller when tasks switch randomly than when they alternate in predictable sequences. They proposed that the difference was due to control mechanisms (anterior attentional network) being activated in the random condition because of its overall difficulty. Besides, it has been shown that increasing arousal levels inhibits the anterior attentional network. Therefore, Tornay and Milán's account predicts that high arousal should result in switch cost for the random condition increasing to the levels of predictable switching. In this work, this prediction was tested by assessing the interaction between increased arousal and switch cost with both predictable and random-task switching. The results may help to solve the ongoing controversy about the causes of switch cost. 相似文献
992.
This study aims to discover whether observers with the same cultural background as the inhabitant of a bedroom perceive common characteristics among the inhabitants. The study was carried out with 107 females who were asked to assess the inhabitants of eight bedrooms. Several slides of each bedroom were shown to participants and they had to choose from a list of seventy-six characteristics those that defined the resident of each bedroom. Results revealed that socio-demographic variables thus attributed coincided with the profiles of the inhabitants. A correspondence analysis brought to light a dimension linked to affective aspects, which was more prominent among women; whereas among men the characteristics thus attributed were more linked to practical aspects and to difficulties for social interaction. A second study was conducted to determine whether gender stereotype had any influence on the result of the previous study. The outcomes showed that this stereotype did not have any effect on the participants' judgments. 相似文献
993.
Christián C. Carman 《国际科学哲学研究》2010,24(3):257-265
In 1975, Imre Lakatos and Elie Zahar claimed that the determination of planetary distances represents excess empirical content of Copernicus’s theory over that of Ptolemy. This claim provoked an interesting discussion during the first half of the 1980s. The discussion started when Alan Chalmers affirmed that it is not correct to attribute this advantage to the Copernican system over the Ptolemaic. Other scholars criticized Chalmers’s assertion, reaffirming the position of Lakatos and Zahar: one went even further, asserting that Copernicus has not one but two methods for calculating distances, even though this claim was subsequently also criticized. But all participants assumed that Ptolemy has no method for calculating planetary distances. In this article, I argue that this is not correct. I argue, in fact, that Ptolemy has two independent methods for calculating the distances of some of the planets and, therefore, as far as the calculation of planetary distances is concerned, Ptolemy’s system surpasses that of Copernicus. 相似文献
994.
Responses to stimuli are faster when the stimulus location spatially corresponds to the required response (standard Simon
effect). Recently, a similar effect has been observed with monocular stimuli. Responses were faster when the response location
and the stimulated eye corresponded (monocular Simon effect). It has been suggested that distinct mechanisms may underlie
these two Simon effects. Here, we attempted to study these two mechanisms simultaneously. For mean reaction time, a finding
of perfect additivity was obtained. These behavioral data coupled with surface electrophysiological measures support the view
that two different mechanisms contribute independently to the monocular and standard Simon effect. 相似文献
995.
The Division of Household Labor in Spanish Dual Earner Couples: Testing Three Theories 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Although the participation of men in household activities has increased, this has not prompted an equitable division of household activities among men and women, since Spanish women continue to perform a greater share of these activities than Spanish men. This article explores the explanatory potential of three theoretical approaches (traditional gender division, role-strain and the resource-bargaining approach) to account for the emergence of different patterns in the division of unpaid work among Spanish dual earner couples. Using a representative sample of 2,877 Spanish workers and through logit ordered models, our study reveals that the three models contribute to the explanation of the different patterns of household labor and, therefore, may be regarded as complementary. 相似文献
996.
Anthony A. Wright Jeffrey S. Katz John Magnotti L. Caitlin Elmore Stephanie Babb Sarah alwin 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2010,17(2):243-249
Six pigeons were trained in a change detection task with four colors. They were shown two colored circles on a sample array,
followed by a test array with the color of one circle changed. The pigeons learned to choose the changed color and transferred
their performance to four unfamiliar colors, suggesting that they had learned a generalized concept of color change. They
also transferred performance to test delays several times their 50-msec training delay without prior delay training. The accurate
delay performance of several seconds suggests that their change detection was memory based, as opposed to a perceptual attentional
capture process. These experiments are the first to show that an animal species (pigeons, in this case) can learn a change
detection task identical to ones used to test human memory, thereby providing the possibility of directly comparing short-term
memory processing across species. 相似文献
997.
998.
Recognition performance does not usually change along the lifespan, but the response criterion usually does, and in general, it changes from being conservative during youth to being liberal, in old age. The focus of the present study is to analyze the changes that take place, both in discrimination and response criterion, as a result of aging in two recognition tasks: one with neutral images, and the other with faces showing positive and negative emotional expressions. Two groups of participants performed both tasks: young (N = 21; age range, 17-33 years), older (N = 21; age range, 65-91 years). The analyses of several discrimination parameters (d' and probability of recognition) and the response criterion yielded significant age differences. Thus, results indicated that the ability to discriminate of older participants was better than that of younger participants when having to recognize neutral images, and faces with negative emotional expressions. The response criterion of younger participants was always conservative, whereas older participants only showed liberal criteria in front of faces with emotional expressions. In relation to the neutral images, the response criterion of older participants was optimum, because it led to more hits, without increasing the false alarms. The results are partially explained by the tasks differential difficulty, and are discussed within the frame of Simulation theory. 相似文献
999.
Figueras Masip A Amador-Campos JA Gómez-Benito J del Barrio Gándara V 《The Spanish journal of psychology》2010,13(2):990-999
The psychometric characteristics of the Children's Depression Inventory, CDI (Kovacs, 1992) in a sample of 1705 participants (792 boys and 913 girls) and a clinical sample of 102 participants (42 boys and 60 girls) between 10 and 18 years old are presented. Reliability coefficients range, for both samples, from .82 (test) to .84 (retest) in the community sample, and .85 (test, clinical sample); test-retest reliability is .81 in the community sample. The mean scores are similar to other Spanish and English ones. Girls score higher than boys. The cut-off point that best differentiates between depressive and community participants is 19, with a sensitivity of 94.7%, a specificity of 95.6%, a positive predictive value of .90, and a negative predictive value of .98. 相似文献
1000.