首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   147篇
  免费   0篇
  147篇
  2019年   1篇
  2013年   4篇
  2012年   1篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   1篇
  2005年   1篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   1篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   1篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   1篇
  1997年   1篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   1篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   4篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   4篇
  1971年   4篇
  1970年   5篇
  1969年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   3篇
  1965年   2篇
  1964年   1篇
  1963年   1篇
排序方式: 共有147条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Memory bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD).   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
There is a memory bias associated with depression, and good reason to expect a memory bias associated with anxiety. However, the results of studies reported to date have been ambiguous. Accordingly, an experiment was conducted to assess memory for contamination in people with different types of anxiety. Memory for contaminated stimuli among participants who met DSM-IV criteria for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and indicated a fear of contamination (n = 10) was compared to memory in a group of anxious controls (n = 10), and in undergraduate students (n = 20). Participants were shown 50 objects, 25 of which were contaminated by the experimenter and 25 which were touched but not contaminated. They then completed a neuropsychological memory assessment, after which the participants were asked to recall all of the objects touched by the experimenter. They were then asked to approach each object and to rate their anxiety about touching it. Finally, participants were asked about their perceptions of the cleanliness of each object. The OCD group had better memory for contaminated objects than for clean ones. Neither control group showed such a bias. Neuropsychological test scores indicated that this bias is not the result of differences in general memory ability. The results are discussed in terms of the memory-deficit theory of OCD and of behavioural and cognitive approaches to understanding the role of information processing in fear and anxiety.  相似文献   
102.
In a laboratory investigation of compulsive urges (and the associated discomfort) it was confirmed that these phenomena are subject to spontaneously-occurring decrements. These decrements take place within 15–180 min, according to variations in the experimental conditions. The declines in urges and discomfort follow parallel courses. With repetition, the declines in urges and discomfort take place more rapidly. The decremental effects are cumulative and stable. The spontaneous decay curve shrinks with repeated trials.  相似文献   
103.
The weaknesses of the equipotentiality premise are rehearsed and an alternative theory of preparedness, with particular reference to phobias, is outlined. Two clinical cases which appear to be contrary to prediction are described. Although their phobias were unprepared (lack of biological significance, rarity, and probably gradual acquisition) they showed features (high resistance and broad generalization) not predicted by the theory. It is suggested that these features may appear as a result of (i) overlearning. (ii) symbolic transformation or (iii) considerable associated psychopathology.  相似文献   
104.
105.
106.
Two groups of agoraphobic patients were treated with one of two forms of exposure-based therapy. In one group, the patients stayed in the target situation until their self-rated anxiety dropped by half (Endurance condition). In the other, the patients were required to leave the fear situation when their anxiety reached a high pre-set level, without waiting for anxiety to drop (Escape condition). The patients in both groups improved, in contrast to those in a Waiting-list control group. There were no significant differences between the two treated groups. The results are discussed in terms of Mowrer's theory of fear and avoidance from which it can be predicted that patients in an ‘escape condition’ should not improve. Contrary to a Mowrerian prediction, patients who were taught to escape when still fearful, did not show increased avoidance behaviour.  相似文献   
107.
Boyd and Levis (1983, this issue, pp. 143–149) have contested the main argument of our earlier paper on exposure and fear-reduction (de Silva and Rachman, 1981). In this reply, our original argument is briefly restated and the position explained. Boyd and Levis' argument that exposure is necessary, is critically examined and it is concluded that the search for alternative (and additional) explanations of fear-reduction is a valid and useful exercise. Further investigations of the mechanisms of exposure and of non-exposure methods, are warranted.  相似文献   
108.
Sixty-nine phobic and eighty-two obsessional patients, treated at the Maudsley Hospital, were rated for ‘preparedness’, the evolutionary significance of the content and behaviour of the disorder. Reliable ratings (r = 0.78 and 0.90) of the dangerousness of the object or situation to pretechnological man indicated that the content of the large majority of the phobias and obsessions are judged as evolutionarily significant. Degree of preparedness, however, did not predict outcome of therapy, suddenness of onset of the disorder, severity of impairment, intensiveness of the treatment received, or age of onset. Nor was there any significant relationship between preparedness and certain other variables in the obsessional sample: stimulus generalization, effect on life style, impaired reproductive capacity and abnormal personality. The implications of these findings for the hypothesis that human phobias and obsessions are prepared, and for the clinical usefulness of the concept of preparedness, are discussed.  相似文献   
109.
110.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号