首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   121篇
  免费   2篇
  123篇
  2020年   2篇
  2015年   2篇
  2013年   3篇
  2012年   2篇
  2011年   4篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   3篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   4篇
  2006年   2篇
  2005年   3篇
  2003年   1篇
  2002年   3篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   3篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   2篇
  1979年   2篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   1篇
  1970年   4篇
  1969年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
  1960年   1篇
  1959年   1篇
  1958年   2篇
  1957年   3篇
  1956年   1篇
  1955年   3篇
  1954年   2篇
  1953年   2篇
  1949年   2篇
排序方式: 共有123条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
32.
Mutual facial recognition was studied in thirty families with and without a disturbed member. It appears that families with a markedly disturbed child have idiosyncratic modes of mutual recognition, particularly in the case of the mother, whereas families without a disturbed member or with a moderately disturbed child do not display idiosyncratic patterns. Although the sample is very small, there is a suggestion that families with a disturbed parent have patterns of recognition that are entirely different from those of the other families in the study.  相似文献   
33.
34.
For one group of ducklings distress calls were reinforced using brief presentation of an imprinted stimulus as the response contingent (reinforcing) event. For a second group of ducklings presentation of an imprinted stimulus was contingent upon a more traditional operant (the pecking response). Regardless of which response was being reinforced, the same pattern ensued. Bursts of responding were separated by periods of little or no responding. When subsequently subjected to repeated presentation and withdrawal of the imprinted stimulus under conditions in which neither response was being reinforced, ducklings with a history of reinforcement for distress calls emitted many more calls than ducklings with a history of reinforcement for pole pecks.  相似文献   
35.
36.
37.
    
Hans Urs von Balthasar's “Theo‐Drama: Theological Dramatic Theory” exhibits a mutual funding of a hierarchical ordering of the relation between “man” and “woman” and a hierarchical ordering of the relation between the Father, the Son, and the Spirit. The two hierarchies explain, illustrate, and support one another. Von Balthasar's oscillation between hierarchy and equality, particularly in the divine case, results in a tortured understanding of personhood where being in relation means handing oneself over to another with the threat of death always present. Von Balthasar's understanding of personhood turns out to be fundamentally masochistic. Further, difference collapses into hierarchy and thus turns out to be no more than repetition in the mode of reception, which then poses a serious challenge to Balthasar's account of divine and human being. Since the point of connection between the two is found in his account of the way inner‐trinitarian relations of origin are extended into the world in the sending of the Son, this is the thesis which needs problematizing. For von Balthasar, the kenotic nature of the inner‐trinitarian processions explains what in the life of God makes the cross possible, but this move ascribes something like suffering and death to the inner life of God in a way that undercuts the fullness of divine love while undergirding a hierarchical understanding of divine relationality.  相似文献   
38.
39.
40.
    
Twenty years after Abraham Maslow's death (in 1970), it is time for a close look at his legacy for the counseling field. Maslow's innovative work, often misinterpreted by both his admirers as well as his critics, remains highly relevant to current concerns in counseling.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号