全文获取类型
收费全文 | 41510篇 |
免费 | 802篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
42320篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 423篇 |
2019年 | 464篇 |
2018年 | 654篇 |
2017年 | 693篇 |
2016年 | 713篇 |
2015年 | 531篇 |
2014年 | 639篇 |
2013年 | 3006篇 |
2012年 | 1127篇 |
2011年 | 1247篇 |
2010年 | 797篇 |
2009年 | 728篇 |
2008年 | 1053篇 |
2007年 | 986篇 |
2006年 | 906篇 |
2005年 | 773篇 |
2004年 | 769篇 |
2003年 | 758篇 |
2002年 | 773篇 |
2001年 | 1275篇 |
2000年 | 1255篇 |
1999年 | 922篇 |
1998年 | 425篇 |
1997年 | 437篇 |
1996年 | 406篇 |
1992年 | 803篇 |
1991年 | 752篇 |
1990年 | 763篇 |
1989年 | 724篇 |
1988年 | 725篇 |
1987年 | 664篇 |
1986年 | 731篇 |
1985年 | 708篇 |
1984年 | 596篇 |
1983年 | 529篇 |
1981年 | 399篇 |
1979年 | 597篇 |
1978年 | 485篇 |
1977年 | 403篇 |
1976年 | 408篇 |
1975年 | 518篇 |
1974年 | 660篇 |
1973年 | 649篇 |
1972年 | 537篇 |
1971年 | 534篇 |
1970年 | 477篇 |
1969年 | 536篇 |
1968年 | 621篇 |
1967年 | 547篇 |
1966年 | 573篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
In two experiments, key-peck responding of pigeons was compared under variable-interval schedules that arranged immediate reinforcement and ones that arranged unsignaled delays of reinforcement. Responses during the nominal unsignaled delay periods had no effect on the reinforcer presentations. In Experiment 1, the unsignaled delays were studied using variable-interval schedules as baselines. Relative to the immediate reinforcement condition, 0.5-s unsignaled delays decreased the duration of the reinforced interresponse times and increased the overall frequency of short (<0.5-s) interresponse times. Longer, 5.0-s unsignaled delays increased the duration of the reinforced interresponse times and decreased the overall frequency of the short interresponse times. In Experiment 2, similar effects to those of Experiment 1 were obtained when the 0.5-s unsignaled delays were imposed upon a baseline schedule that explicitly arranged reinforcement of short interresponse times and therefore already generated a large number of short interresponse times. The results support earlier suggestions that the unsignaled 0.5-s delays change the functional response unit from a single key peck to a multiple key-peck unit. These findings are discussed in terms of the mechanisms by which contingencies control response structure in the absence of specific structural requirements. 相似文献
22.
The effects of two alternative sources of food delivery on the key-peck responding of pigeons were examined. Pecking was maintained by a variable-interval 3-min schedule. In the presence of this schedule in different conditions, either a variable-time 3-min schedule delivering food independently of responding or an equivalent schedule that required a minimum 2-s pause between a key peck and food delivery (a differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule) was added. The differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule reduced response rates more than did the variable-time schedule in most instances. The delay between a key peck and the next reinforcer consistently was longer under the differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule than under the variable-time schedule. Response rates and median delay between responses and reinforcers were negatively correlated. These results contradict earlier conclusions about the behavioral effects of alternative reinforcement. They suggest that an interpretation in terms of response–reinforcer contiguity is consistent with the data. 相似文献
23.
Rats were allowed to forage in a simulated natural environment made up of eight food sources (patches) each containing a fixed number of pellets. Two of the eight contained an extra supply of peanuts. The peanut patches were signaled by an olfactory/visual cue located at the bottom of the ladder leading to the patch. In successive phases the number of sessions per day, height of the patches, and availability of peanuts were manipulated. Subjects showed evidence of discrimination learning under these conditions, although the degree of discriminatory behavior varied as a function of environmental manipulations. Assessment of behavior within foraging sessions showed that subjects systematically changed their patterns of utilization of patches across time. Sampling or exploration, as well as food reinforcement, seem implicated in these results. 相似文献
24.
25.
Body-frame size is an important factor in determining an optimal body weight for a given height. Previous studies have indicated that many individuals incorrectly estimate their body-frame size, and, as a result, incorrectly assess their ideal weight. The present study investigated the accuracy of estimation of body-frame size as a function of sex and actual frame size. The subjects were 66 men and 52 women participating in a community adult fitness program. Data indicated that medium-framed individuals were the most accurate in their estimations of body-frame size. Also, women were twice as likely to be accurate as were men. These results are interpreted to mean that most people assume they are medium-framed and that there is a sex difference in the way body-frame size is estimated. 相似文献
26.
This research investigated the buffering role of social support in the relationship of work load to both tension-anxiety and coping for police radio dispatchers ( N = 60). Each dispatcher was observed throughout an entire 8-hour work shift by a trained observer. Objective load consisted of the hourly rate of incoming telephone calls, police radio transmissions, and face-to-face or written communications from superiors, co-workers, and others. At the end of each shift, measures were taken of dispatchers' perceived load, felt tension-anxiety, and coping actions. Hierarchical regression revealed that support from superiors moderated the effects of both objective and perceived loads; no main effects of support were found. Statistically significant interactions confirmed the hypothesized buffering role of support: under high perceived load, dispatchers with high social support engaged in more coping actions and felt less tension-anxiety than did low-support dispatchers. No differences were found when perceived load was low. Support also buffered effects of objective load on tension-anxiety but not on coping. 相似文献
27.
K Friemert 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1988,40(11):671-677
Basing on the author's experiences during his study stay at the Serbsky All-Union Research Institute for General and Forensic Psychiatry in Moscow a report is given about the theoretical foundations and the carrying-out of forensic-psychiatric expert-opinions in the field of penal as well as civil law in the U.S.S.R. Some peculiarities in comparison with the practice in the G.D.R. are taken in special account. 相似文献
28.
29.
S Martin K Housley H Mccoy P Greenhouse F Stigger M A Kenney S Shoffner V Fu M Korslund F G Ercanli-Huffman 《Perceptual and motor skills》1988,67(3):879-884
The Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale was administered to 550 14- and 16-yr.-old (+/- 6 mo.) girls. Self-esteem scores were categorized by weight and weight by height. Scores on the Quetelet Index for obesity were correlated with self-esteem scores. Mean self-esteem of the low- and middle-weight by height group was significantly higher than the mean of the high-weight by height group. In analyzing weight alone, the self-esteem of the middle-weight group was significantly higher than the self-esteem of the high-weight group. The correlation of the obesity index and self-esteem indicated that as weight increased self-esteem decreased. 相似文献
30.
S W?sser W Handrick F B Spencker W Braun 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1988,40(2):95-101
Observations on three children with neurolisteriosis (one case of meningitis, two cases of meningoencephalitis, each Serovar 4 b), show that even after the neonatal period, listeriosis must not be ignored in the process of diagnosis and therapy. It is the bacteriological examination of the cerebrospinal fluid, together with the blood culture, and not clinical symptoms and serology that guarantee a timely diagnosis and therapy (ampicillin and gentamicin). 相似文献