全文获取类型
收费全文 | 25635篇 |
免费 | 480篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
26117篇 |
出版年
2018年 | 326篇 |
2017年 | 353篇 |
2016年 | 370篇 |
2015年 | 266篇 |
2014年 | 336篇 |
2013年 | 1763篇 |
2012年 | 590篇 |
2011年 | 642篇 |
2010年 | 442篇 |
2009年 | 402篇 |
2008年 | 621篇 |
2007年 | 558篇 |
2006年 | 536篇 |
2005年 | 458篇 |
2004年 | 463篇 |
2003年 | 465篇 |
2002年 | 473篇 |
2001年 | 645篇 |
2000年 | 645篇 |
1999年 | 510篇 |
1998年 | 257篇 |
1997年 | 284篇 |
1996年 | 282篇 |
1995年 | 270篇 |
1992年 | 494篇 |
1991年 | 459篇 |
1990年 | 455篇 |
1989年 | 430篇 |
1988年 | 451篇 |
1987年 | 417篇 |
1986年 | 465篇 |
1985年 | 439篇 |
1984年 | 392篇 |
1983年 | 349篇 |
1982年 | 265篇 |
1981年 | 285篇 |
1979年 | 423篇 |
1978年 | 335篇 |
1977年 | 288篇 |
1976年 | 298篇 |
1975年 | 348篇 |
1974年 | 445篇 |
1973年 | 472篇 |
1972年 | 352篇 |
1971年 | 377篇 |
1970年 | 341篇 |
1969年 | 366篇 |
1968年 | 437篇 |
1967年 | 402篇 |
1966年 | 417篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
David Menendez Karl S. Rosengren Martha W. Alibali 《Applied cognitive psychology》2020,34(5):1101-1117
People often have difficulty in understanding processes of biological change, and they typically reject drastic life cycle changes such as metamorphosis, except for animals with which they are familiar. Even after a lesson about metamorphosis, people often do not generalize to animals not seen during the lesson. This might be partially due to the perceptual richness of the diagrams typically used during lessons on metamorphosis, which serves to emphasize the individual animal rather than a class of animals. In two studies, we examined whether the perceptual richness of a diagram influences adults' learning and transfer of knowledge about metamorphosis. One study was conducted in a laboratory setting, and the other was online. In both studies, adults who saw the bland diagram during the lesson accurately transferred more than adults who saw the rich diagram during the lesson. 相似文献
922.
Charmaine W.Q. Lim Al K.C. Au Rhiannon N. Turner 《Journal of community & applied social psychology》2020,30(3):307-321
Imagined contact can be effective at reducing social stigma. However, the effect may depend on the strength of the stigma held. We tested the robustness of imagined contact in an Asian setting where stigmatization of mental illness is stronger than in Western countries. In Experiment 1 (n = 167) with five conditions, only an enhanced version of positive imagined contact was able to decrease stigma towards people with schizophrenia through decreasing intergroup anxiety. Given the potential discrepancy between imaginations and reality about experiences with stigmatized groups, in Experiment 2 (n = 121), we tested the hypothesis that after presenting participants with factual information about a mental illness group, imagined contact might backfire, resulting in more negative perceptions. However, enhanced imagined contact alongside factual message about schizophrenia did not increase stigma. The backfiring hypothesis was therefore not supported. Nevertheless, providing realistic information did negate the positive effects of enhanced imagined contact on stigma reduction. In both experiments, we also showed that intergroup anxiety mediated the effect of enhanced imagined contact on various measures of stigma. 相似文献
923.
Incremental validity testing (i.e., testing whether a focal predictor is associated with an outcome above and beyond a covariate) is common (e.g., 57% of Personal Relationships articles in 2017), yet it is fraught with conceptual and statistical problems. First, researchers often use it to overemphasize the novelty or counterintuitiveness of findings, which hinders cumulative understanding. Second, incremental validity testing requires that the focal predictor and the covariate represent separate constructs; researchers risk committing the “jangle fallacy” without such evidence. Third, the most common approach to incremental validity testing (i.e., standard multiple regression, 88% of articles) inflates Type I error and can produce invalid conclusions. This article also discusses the relevance of these issues to dyadic/longitudinal designs and offers concrete solutions. 相似文献
924.
Thomas D.W. Wilcockson Edwin J. Burns Baiqiang Xia Jeremy Tree Trevor J. Crawford 《Visual cognition》2020,28(4):311-323
ABSTRACT When people recognize faces, they normally move their eyes so that their first fixation is in the optimal location for efficient perceptual processing. This location is found just below the centre-point between the eyes. This type of attentional bias could be partly innate, but also an inevitable developmental process that aids our ability to recognize faces. We investigated whether a group of people with developmental prosopagnosia would also demonstrate neurotypical first fixation locations when recognizing faces during an eye-tracking task. We found evidence that adults with prosopagnosia had atypically heterogeneous first fixations in comparison to controls. However, differences were limited to the vertical, but not horizontal, plane of the face. We interpret these findings by suggesting that subtle changes to face-based eye movement patterns in developmental prosopagnosia may underpin their face recognition impairments, and suggest future work is still needed to address this possibility. 相似文献
925.
926.
Jack W. Klein Garrett Tyler-Parker Brock Bastian 《Australian journal of psychology》2020,72(3):276-282
927.
Scott Parker Robert W. Jernigan Joshua M. Lansky 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2020,73(1):170-183
The Wilcoxon–Mann–Whitney procedure is invariant under monotone transformations but its use as a test of location or shift is said not to be so. It tests location only under the shift model, the assumption of parallel cumulative distribution functions (cdfs). We show that infinitely many monotone transformations of the measured variable produce parallel cdfs, so long as the original cdfs intersect nowhere or everywhere. Thus there are infinitely many effect sizes measured as shifts of medians, invalidating the notion that there is one true shift parameter and thereby rendering any single estimate dubious. Measuring effect size using the probability of superiority alleviates this difficulty. 相似文献
928.
929.
930.
Marta Doroszuk Maria Magdalena Kwiatkowska Jorge Torres-Marín Ginés Navarro-Carrillo Anna Włodarczyk Ana Blasco-Belled Laura Martínez-Buelvas James David Albert Newton Oscar Oviedo-Trespalacios Radosław Rogoza 《International journal of psychology》2020,55(3):413-424
A recent re-operationalisation of grandiose narcissism has resulted in the distinction of two narcissistic strategies based on the cognitive, affective-motivational and behavioural dynamics: admiration (assertive self-enhancement) and rivalry (antagonistic self-protection). The Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Questionnaire (NARQ) was developed to assess this model with two higher-order dimensions. However, cross-validations of the NARQ have not been extensively conducted across diverse population groups and languages. This study aimed to test the internal and external validity (through the relation with envy and self-esteem), reliability and cross-cultural equivalence of the Spanish version of the NARQ. The psychometric properties were evaluated in a Spanish sample (N = 310), and cross-cultural equivalence was tested in participants from Chile (N = 234) and Colombia (N = 256). The results supported the reliability and validity of the Spanish NARQ, as well as the cross-cultural equivalence across Spanish-speaking countries. In addition, we discuss obtained differences across Spanish, Chilean and Colombian sample within two narcissistic strategies. 相似文献