全文获取类型
收费全文 | 39346篇 |
免费 | 1608篇 |
国内免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
40968篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 364篇 |
2019年 | 424篇 |
2018年 | 591篇 |
2017年 | 600篇 |
2016年 | 666篇 |
2015年 | 498篇 |
2014年 | 574篇 |
2013年 | 2678篇 |
2012年 | 1118篇 |
2011年 | 1087篇 |
2010年 | 684篇 |
2009年 | 653篇 |
2008年 | 964篇 |
2007年 | 962篇 |
2006年 | 835篇 |
2005年 | 812篇 |
2004年 | 749篇 |
2003年 | 690篇 |
2002年 | 714篇 |
2001年 | 1273篇 |
2000年 | 1234篇 |
1999年 | 891篇 |
1998年 | 402篇 |
1997年 | 352篇 |
1992年 | 867篇 |
1991年 | 815篇 |
1990年 | 822篇 |
1989年 | 716篇 |
1988年 | 718篇 |
1987年 | 689篇 |
1986年 | 723篇 |
1985年 | 792篇 |
1984年 | 592篇 |
1983年 | 545篇 |
1982年 | 370篇 |
1981年 | 364篇 |
1979年 | 690篇 |
1978年 | 430篇 |
1977年 | 420篇 |
1976年 | 412篇 |
1975年 | 587篇 |
1974年 | 670篇 |
1973年 | 715篇 |
1972年 | 618篇 |
1971年 | 578篇 |
1970年 | 574篇 |
1969年 | 561篇 |
1968年 | 751篇 |
1967年 | 664篇 |
1966年 | 595篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Perceptual sensitivity to dimensional and similarity relations in free and rule-governed classifications 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Two experiments were conducted to investigate the perceptual primacy of dimensional and similarity relations in the stimulus classifications of younger and older subjects. In Experiment 1, 4- and 10-year-olds were given free classifications in which they could group stimuli according to overall similarity or identities in size, color, or orientation. Both age groups classified stimuli most frequently according to identities on separate dimensions. In Experiment 2, 4-year-olds and adults were given free classifications followed by rule-governed classifications which required them to group stimuli according to specific relations. In the free classifications, a majority of subjects in both age groups classified the stimuli most frequently according to identities on separate dimensions. In the rule-governed classifications, both age groups were more accurate when a single separate relation was required for solution than when overall similarity was required. These results support a differential-sensitivity view of perceptual development, which asserts that individuals at all ages primarily perceive and use separate relations. 相似文献
102.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
103.
Using a multi-trial, free-recall paradigm (Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; Lezak, 1983), the acquisition and retention by learning disabled (LD) young adults with a Halstead Impairment Index less than or equal to 0.3 (low LD) or greater than or equal to 0.4 (high LD) was compared with that of nondisabled and head-injured (HI) young adults. Significant group differences, particularly during the last three acquisition trials, confirmed the effectiveness of this method in revealing subtle learning and retention deficits. Analyses of Trial 1 recall showed group differences in the prerecency but not the recency portion of the serial position curve, thus implicating long-term but not short-term memory differences between the groups. Analyses of pair frequency (Sternberg & Tulving, 1977) data showed that the three clinical groups had difficulty organizing the to-be-learned material for effective retrieval. Following an interpolated task, retention was poorer for the clinical groups than for the nondisabled and varied inversely with the degree of neuropsychological impairment. Finally, during retention, the nondisabled and low LD groups retained and used the list organization established during acquisition whereas the high LD and HI groups did not. 相似文献
104.
Subjects were presented with word pairs (e.g., bed-mattress) and timed as they decided whether one word named part of the item named by the other word. Yes responses were facilitated, and no responses were impeded, by relation similarity (i.e., the similarity of the relation between the two stimulus items to the part-whole relation). Item similarity, the similarity of the two stimulus items to each other, had no effect. The results support the view that similarity effects in semantic decision tasks are due to the comparison of elements of the relation between the two stimulus words against relation elements that serve as criteria for the target relation that the subject has been asked to identify. 相似文献
105.
Thirty pediatricians and 30 parents were interviewed on (a) how and when children develop concepts about death, (b) children's emotional responses to the thought of death, (c) the possibility of dialogue between adults and children about death, and (d) differences in healthy and seriously ill children's conceptualization of death. Pediatricians were no different than parents in their opinions. Moreover, in some cases, less educated parents and older doctors tended to hold opinions that were even farther from reality. Overall, the results suggest that children, healthy or sick, are left alone with the problem of death and are not helped either by their parents or by their doctors. 相似文献
106.
107.
Training in systemic therapy necessitates a structured approach to teaching systemic thinking. The Systemic Therapy Sessions Summary Form, a record-keeping method, is presented as a tool for enhancing training in systemic therapy. The purpose of this form is to provide the structure for the trainee to develop circular hypotheses, to connect reflexively the assessment and intervention components of therapy, and to maintain a sense of the evolving nature of therapy across sessions. A case example illustrates the use of this record-keeping format. 相似文献
108.
109.
110.
Amnesia as a consequence of male rape: a case report 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2