全文获取类型
收费全文 | 58607篇 |
免费 | 1271篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
59884篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 536篇 |
2019年 | 649篇 |
2018年 | 906篇 |
2017年 | 939篇 |
2016年 | 1022篇 |
2015年 | 780篇 |
2014年 | 882篇 |
2013年 | 4159篇 |
2012年 | 1633篇 |
2011年 | 1616篇 |
2010年 | 1017篇 |
2009年 | 965篇 |
2008年 | 1400篇 |
2007年 | 1445篇 |
2006年 | 1229篇 |
2005年 | 1177篇 |
2004年 | 1101篇 |
2003年 | 1044篇 |
2002年 | 1081篇 |
2001年 | 1857篇 |
2000年 | 1780篇 |
1999年 | 1328篇 |
1998年 | 588篇 |
1996年 | 537篇 |
1992年 | 1214篇 |
1991年 | 1152篇 |
1990年 | 1176篇 |
1989年 | 1031篇 |
1988年 | 1037篇 |
1987年 | 980篇 |
1986年 | 1030篇 |
1985年 | 1109篇 |
1984年 | 852篇 |
1983年 | 782篇 |
1982年 | 545篇 |
1981年 | 542篇 |
1979年 | 969篇 |
1978年 | 660篇 |
1977年 | 607篇 |
1976年 | 585篇 |
1975年 | 823篇 |
1974年 | 964篇 |
1973年 | 1015篇 |
1972年 | 850篇 |
1971年 | 805篇 |
1970年 | 800篇 |
1969年 | 760篇 |
1968年 | 1014篇 |
1967年 | 885篇 |
1966年 | 788篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
T.B. Rogers 《Acta psychologica》1978,42(1):21-28
The process of responding to attitude items was broken down into a series of cognitive stages and a model offered. To test this model, subjects responded to attitude items varying in Extremity under two or five response alternative formats. By measuring response times, and applying Sternberg's (1969) additive factor method, the model was supported. The results were discussed in terms of previous process work involving personality items and sentence verification tasks. 相似文献
922.
923.
Geoffrey R. Loftus 《Memory & cognition》1978,6(4):416-422
Two experiments investigated the processes by which compass directions are comprehended. In both experiments, the task was as follows. A compass direction (e.g., 210 deg) was visually presented. The subject drew a representation of the direction and then pressed a key. Reaction time from onset of the stimulus to the keypress was measured. The results suggested a model of direction comprehension involving two steps: first, computing the nearest cardinal direction to the target direction, and then “rotating” from the cardinal to the target direction. Rotation could be performed equally well clockwise or counterclockwise. Additionally, north-south. tended to be comprehended faster than east-west, confirming past results that have shown an advantage of up-down over left-right in discrimination tasks. 相似文献
924.
Barbara B. Domangue 《Journal of personality》1978,46(3):519-535
The effects of inconsistency between the verbal and nonverbal components of a message were investigated. Subjects were 72 females who rated the attitudes of encoding confederates who communicated either consistent or inconsistent messages. Verbal and nonverbal components were varied as either positive or negative and each of the four possible combinations served as an experimental condition. The nonverbal cues of amount of eye contact, postural orientation, forward–backward lean, and head nodding were found to influence the judgment of the total message. Verbal and nonverbal components were found to have interactive rather than additive effects. Inconsistent nonverbal cues had a stronger effect when accompanying a positive verbal message than when accompanying a negative verbal component. The effects of decoders' levels of cognitive complexity and tolerance of ambiguity were also assessed. Evidence suggests that both complexity and ambiguity tolerance have effects on the utilization of inconsistent nonverbal cues. Low complexity/low tolerance subjects were less influenced by nonverbal cues in the positive verbal–negative nonverbal condition. These subjects were also found to differentiate less between supplied rating dimensions than other groups. 相似文献
925.
REM motivation induced by brief REM deprivation: the influence of cognition, gender, and personality. 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
D B Cohen M J McGarth L W Bell M J Hanlon N Simon 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1978,36(7):741-751
Prominence of the brain's right-hemisphere information processing and intensity of dream experience are two theoretically related constructs that have been proposed as important psychological aspects of REM sleep. Either view is consistent with the prediction that the effect of REM deprivation will depend in part on the nature of cognitive activity that is initiated at the onset of each REM period and that "substitutes" for the interrupted REM process. In the present study, the effect of REM deprivation was more striking for female subjects given a digits task than for female subjects given a fantasy-reporting task during awakening used to induce REM deprivation for the first 6 hours of the night. High neuroticism appeared to exaggerate the effect. No corresponding pattern was observed for the male subjects. These preliminary finding may be exaggerated by cognitive activity that is functionally incongruent with those processes. The results also raise interesting questions about individual differences. 相似文献
926.
This study partially replicated Martens' (1969a) social-facilitation study of motor behavior. His very robust performance findings provided impressive confirmation for Zajonc's hypothesis, and his arousal findings have since been used as evidence for a nonlearned-drive basis for social facilitation. The present study also extended Martens' investigation by examining the separate and combined effects of an audience and videotape camera. The effects due to the presence of the audience and camera were not additive; instead, the audience detrimentally affected subjects' performance consistency and the camera resulted in more trials with errors greater than 30 msec after the performance criteria had been attained. Martens' most robust findings for constant error were not replicated, nor were some of his physiological arousal findings. His pattern of constant error results over all trials is atypical of known learning strategies that subjects use to reduce error over successive trials. Overall, audience effects accounted for only a very small portion of the variance. 相似文献
927.
A sexual sadist was treated by an olfactory aversion procedure for eight weeks. Pre-treatment assessment of sexual arousal showed high levels of response to sadistic stimuli as well as high arousal to non-sadistic heterosexual stimuli. In treatment the inhalation of the gas of an odoriferous chemical agent, valeric acid, was paired with slide presentations of sadistic materials while penile erection was monitored. The aversive stimulus immediately and permanently suppressed the response for the duration of treatment. In separate measurement sessions, with valeric acid absent, deviant response remained suppressed and nondeviant response was unaffected. Post-treatment reassessment revealed very low levels of response to sadistic stimuli. Follow-up sessions showed that the deviant response was absent eight months following the conclusion of treatment. 相似文献
928.
H.B. Gibson 《Behaviour research and therapy》1978,16(3):191-195
Certain of the states commonly diagnosed by psychiatrists as “affective disorder” do not respond favourably to the usual treatments prescribed, and it is argued by Lum (1976) that such states are not appropriately labelled, as they are primarily due to the somatic condition of hypocapnoea—an abnormally low level of arterial carbon dioxide. This condition is well-recognized and can be diagnosed by appropriate physiological investigation. Hypocapnoea seriously affects various somatic systems, and if the patient later experiences affective distress it is a secondary symptom due to a vicious circle mediated by autonomic responses. The treatment of choice is a form of behaviour therapy aimed at altering the breathing response in the direction of building up toleration of a higher habitual level of arterial carbon dioxide. Existing work by behaviour therapists and others on the physiological and behavioural responses to varying levels of arterial carbon dioxide is cited, and the rationale of symptomatic treatment is discussed. Current treatment methods for hypocapnoea, although behaviourally oriented, are not closely in accord with the principles of modern behaviour therapy, and this paper suggests some directions of improvement. 相似文献
929.
930.