全文获取类型
收费全文 | 87004篇 |
免费 | 1657篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
88669篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 1035篇 |
2019年 | 1283篇 |
2018年 | 1518篇 |
2017年 | 1517篇 |
2016年 | 1834篇 |
2015年 | 1355篇 |
2014年 | 1406篇 |
2013年 | 6708篇 |
2012年 | 2542篇 |
2011年 | 2671篇 |
2010年 | 1646篇 |
2009年 | 1598篇 |
2008年 | 2419篇 |
2007年 | 2390篇 |
2006年 | 2087篇 |
2005年 | 1939篇 |
2004年 | 1870篇 |
2003年 | 1705篇 |
2002年 | 1759篇 |
2001年 | 2693篇 |
2000年 | 2535篇 |
1999年 | 1947篇 |
1998年 | 996篇 |
1997年 | 868篇 |
1996年 | 812篇 |
1993年 | 790篇 |
1992年 | 1692篇 |
1991年 | 1569篇 |
1990年 | 1584篇 |
1989年 | 1398篇 |
1988年 | 1360篇 |
1987年 | 1338篇 |
1986年 | 1376篇 |
1985年 | 1482篇 |
1984年 | 1164篇 |
1983年 | 1051篇 |
1979年 | 1207篇 |
1978年 | 856篇 |
1977年 | 780篇 |
1976年 | 808篇 |
1975年 | 1077篇 |
1974年 | 1165篇 |
1973年 | 1199篇 |
1972年 | 1070篇 |
1971年 | 975篇 |
1970年 | 927篇 |
1969年 | 908篇 |
1968年 | 1177篇 |
1967年 | 1097篇 |
1966年 | 979篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
Three experiments are reported that represent a reexamination of the missing-number method (Buschke, 1963b) of estimating short-term memory span. The missing-number task involved presenting a random sequence of all but one of the numbers of a known reference set and asking subjects to identify the missing number. Experiment 1 introduced a modified missing-number task that included two missing items and two choices made by the subject. With a large decline in performance for the second choice relative to the first, it is possible that only the second choice was subject to output or retrieval interference. An alternative explanation is that subjects output the number with the weakest memory representation as their first response. By postcuing subjects to report their two choices in a forward or backward sequence, Experiment 2 provided evidence against the importance of output interference and support either for the importance of retrieval interference or for the "weakest-first" hypothesis. However, with a paradigm that replaced only correctly identified missing numbers, a prediction that subjects would select the number with the weakest memory representation as their first response was not confirmed in Experiment 3. Instead, retrieval interference was implicated to explain the first-choice superiority found in Experiments 1 and 3. The results were interpreted in terms of the TODAM model of Murdock (1982, 1987, in press). 相似文献
172.
B P McCloskey R L Klatzky J W Pellegrino 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(4):948-961
Previous research (Klatzky et al., 1989) shows that the time required to make sensible/nonsensible judgments about an action-object phrase (e.g., "rub your stomach") is reliably faster when the phrase is preceded by a cure representing a specific prototypical hand shape (vs. a neutral cue). The current experiments investigated the effects of preparing for an alternate task (finger tapping vs. syllable vocalization) on facilitatory priming of sensibility judgments. Preparation for finger tapping reduced the magnitude of the priming effect more than preparation for vocalization, suggesting that resources accessed during semantic processing of action-object phrases are also used during manual response preparation. The results support the existence of a system representing manual actions that is limited in the number of activities that can be represented at one time and that is not so general that it represents manual and vocal tract movements. 相似文献
173.
Musical duplex perception: perception of figurally good chords with subliminal distinguishing tones.
M D Hall R E Pastore 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(3):752-762
In a variant of duplex perception with speech, phoneme perception is maintained when distinguishing components are presented below intensities required for separate detection, forming the basis for the claim that a phonetic module takes precedence over nonspeech processing. This finding is replicated with music chords (C major and minor) created by mixing a piano fifth with a sinusoidal distinguishing tone (E or E flat). Individual threshold intensities for detecting E or E flat in the context of the fixed piano tones are established. Chord discrimination thresholds defined by distinguishing tone intensity were determined. Experiment 2 verified masked detection thresholds and subliminal chord identification for experienced musicians. Accurate chord perception was maintained at distinguishing tone intensities nearly 20 dB below the threshold for separate detection. Speech and music findings are argued to demonstrate general perceptual principles. 相似文献
174.
Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
M K Kaiser D R Proffitt S M Whelan H Hecht 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(3):669-689
The motions of objects in the environment reflect underlying dynamical constraints and regularities. The conditions under which people are sensitive to natural dynamics are considered. In particular, the article considers what determines whether observers can distinguish canonical and anomalous dynamics when viewing ongoing events. The extent to which such perceptual appreciations are integrated with and influence common-sense reasoning about mechanical events is examined. It is concluded that animation evokes accurate dynamical intuitions when there is only 1 dimension of information that is of dynamical relevance. This advantage is lost when the observed motion reflects higher dimension dynamics or when the kinematic information is removed or degraded. 相似文献
175.
D A Balota S R Black M Cheney 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(2):485-502
Three experiments addressed the distinction between automatic and attentional mechanisms underlying semantic priming effects by factorially crossing prime-target relatedness, expectancy, and SOA in a task (pronunciation) that minimized postlexical checking processes. Also, possible age-related (young vs. older adults) differences in the automatic and attentional mechanisms were addressed. Across all experiments there was evidence of a Relatedness x Expectancy x SOA interaction, which is inconsistent with the notion of independent automatic and attentional mechanisms in semantic priming and the notion of a self-encapsulated modular lexicon. The results also indicated age-related differences in the build-up of the expectancy effect across SOAs when the prime was visually available for only 200 ms, independently of the prime-target SOA (Experiments 1 and 3), but not when the prime was visually available throughout the SOA (Experiment 2). 相似文献
176.
177.
James R. Stagray David Downs Ronald K. Sommers 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1992,21(6):511-524
We measured subjective-tone identification thresholds evoked by acoustically filtered complex tones and compared them to Mandarin-Chinese tone-phoneme identification thresholds previously measured under the same filtering conditions. Tone phonemes were identified at intensities below subjective-tone identification thresholds, suggesting subjective tone is not necessary for tone-phoneme identification. Lower subjective-tone thresholds evoked by resolved harmonics rather than unresolved harmonics were consistent with pattern recognition theories of pitch perception.The authors thank Donald Gans and Richard Klich for all their help. 相似文献
178.
Rats submitted to a chronic variable stress regime exhibited more inactivity during inescapable shock as compared with unstressed rats. Besides, chronic treatments with desipramine, imipramine, clomipramine, or phenelzine reduce the amount of inactivity exhibited during this aversive experience. Furthermore, the combination of both chronic treatments--stress and antidepressants--resulted in a potentiation of the antidepressant effect alone. This higher antidepressant efficacy may result from the interaction of the neural mechanisms triggered by chronic stress exposure and antidepressant drugs. 相似文献
179.
Reports that the dihydropyridine Ca2+ channel antagonists may facilitate memory led to the present study of the behavioral effects of nifedipine. Ninety-two 4-day-old male chicks received 0, 100 nM, 100 microM, or 10 mM nifedipine. Drugs were administered in volumes of 2 microliters into the fourth cerebral ventricle 5 min before training on a visual discrimination task. Nifedipine did not produce any detectable changes in behavior during acquisition trials. Retention, however, was impaired 24 h after training in the 100 nM and 100 microM nifedipine-treated chicks, which made significantly more errors than controls. Nifedipine did not affect the amount of time required to complete the task. No effects on body weight gain were detected, suggesting that the memory impairment was not due to a change in feeding behavior. These data are discussed in terms of the role of calcium-dependent processes in memory. 相似文献
180.
P G Zanone J A Kelso 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(2):403-421
Learning a bimanual coordination task (synchronization to a visually specified phasing relation) was studied as a dynamical process over 5 days of practicing a required phasing pattern. Systematic probes of the attractor layout of the 5 Ss' coordination dynamics (expressed through a collective variable, relative phase) were conducted before, during, and after practice. Depending on the relationship between the initial coordination dynamics (so-called intrinsic dynamics) and the pattern to be learned (termed behavioral information, which acts as an attractor of the coordination dynamics toward the required phasing), qualitative changes in the phase diagram occurred with learning, accompanied by quantitative evidence for loss of stability (phase transitions). Such effects persisted beyond 1 week. The nature of change due to learning (e.g., abrupt vs. gradual) is shown to arise from the cooperative or competitive interplay between behavioral information and the intrinsic dynamics. 相似文献