首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   70788篇
  免费   2834篇
  国内免费   29篇
  2020年   742篇
  2019年   902篇
  2018年   1222篇
  2017年   1293篇
  2016年   1370篇
  2015年   981篇
  2014年   1182篇
  2013年   5272篇
  2012年   2183篇
  2011年   2178篇
  2010年   1397篇
  2009年   1317篇
  2008年   1952篇
  2007年   1952篇
  2006年   1725篇
  2005年   1600篇
  2004年   1486篇
  2003年   1379篇
  2002年   1417篇
  2001年   2175篇
  2000年   2084篇
  1999年   1589篇
  1998年   725篇
  1997年   670篇
  1992年   1443篇
  1991年   1301篇
  1990年   1343篇
  1989年   1180篇
  1988年   1225篇
  1987年   1148篇
  1986年   1207篇
  1985年   1221篇
  1984年   1003篇
  1983年   948篇
  1982年   676篇
  1981年   643篇
  1979年   1150篇
  1978年   771篇
  1977年   718篇
  1976年   686篇
  1975年   952篇
  1974年   1084篇
  1973年   1146篇
  1972年   920篇
  1971年   880篇
  1970年   840篇
  1969年   880篇
  1968年   1109篇
  1967年   1000篇
  1966年   871篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
41.
Subjects in five experiments read nine-digit memory lists from a cathode ray tube for immediate recall. Reading aloud always produced a localized and reliable advantage for the last item, compared to reading silently. Two experiments on whispered and mouthed lists, with or without simultaneous broadband noise, falsified expectations derived from the theory of precategorical acoustic storage. Three additional experiments showed no enhancement of recency in the silent conditions when the digits were drawn or spelled gradually on the screen, a result that is inconsistent with the changing-state hypothesis. The classic auditory-visual modality effect is large and reliable, but still poorly understood.  相似文献   
42.
43.
44.
In two experiments, hungry rats were given instrumental lever-press training for an appetitive reinforcer and, in addition, were exposed to another type of food which was not contingent on lever pressing. In the first experiment, exposure to each type of food was on separate days, whereas in the second experiment rats were exposed to each type of food in strict alternation within each session. Subsequently, a food aversion was conditioned to the reinforcer for the experimental group and to the non-contingent food for the control group. In both experiments, animals with an aversion to the reinforcer responded less in an extinction test than animals with an aversion to the non-contingent food. Subsequent reacquisition tests confirmed that the aversion to the non-contingent food in the control group was of comparable strength with that to the reinforcer in the experimental group. The results were discussed in terms of whether the reinforcer is encoded in the associative structure set up by exposure to an instrumental contingency.  相似文献   
45.
46.
47.
48.
In this essay I address three ways in which Edwards can inform Christian understanding of public life. First I show how Edwards provides both philosophical and theological rationales for social engagement and thereby resists the separation of religion from public life, and use his consideration of poverty as an illustration. Part II examines Edwards's dialectical treatment of patriotism, demonstrating both its importance to the Christian life and its susceptibility to deceptive accommodation to culture. Finally, in Part III I discuss Edwards's use of "national covenant," which despite its temptation to chauvinism Edwards used to undermine national pride. In the conclusion I assess what we can use from Edwards for contemporary Christian understandings of public life.  相似文献   
49.
50.
The Evaluative Space Model of emotions allows for the coactivation of positive‐appetitive and negative‐avoidant systems, but few studies have examined mixed emotions in child development. Existing research suggests children's understanding of opposite valence emotion combinations emerges by approximately 11 years of age. However, it is not yet clear whether various opposite valence combinations are understood at different ages, nor whether children can understand them in others before they have experienced such mixed emotions themselves. Semi‐structured interviews with 97 children investigated whether they regarded six combinations of opposite valence mixed emotions as possible, could provide reasons for them, and report their own experience of each in the context of mother–child relationships. Both understanding that such combinations are possible and ability to provide reasons for them increased after age 6 and up to age 11, but were still incomplete in 12‐year‐olds. Understanding of different opposite valence combinations developed at different rates. At each age, fewer children who showed understanding of these combinations in others reported having had a similar experience themselves. The findings suggest a need to systematically examine a range of mixed emotions in order to develop a comprehensive theory of the development of mixed emotion understanding. They also suggest extending research into adolescence.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号