全文获取类型
收费全文 | 66644篇 |
免费 | 2696篇 |
国内免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
69360篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 614篇 |
2019年 | 786篇 |
2018年 | 1066篇 |
2017年 | 1098篇 |
2016年 | 1147篇 |
2015年 | 856篇 |
2014年 | 1020篇 |
2013年 | 4748篇 |
2012年 | 1946篇 |
2011年 | 1975篇 |
2010年 | 1207篇 |
2009年 | 1168篇 |
2008年 | 1744篇 |
2007年 | 1719篇 |
2006年 | 1561篇 |
2005年 | 1452篇 |
2004年 | 1381篇 |
2003年 | 1260篇 |
2002年 | 1292篇 |
2001年 | 2060篇 |
2000年 | 1922篇 |
1999年 | 1463篇 |
1998年 | 724篇 |
1996年 | 605篇 |
1992年 | 1309篇 |
1991年 | 1230篇 |
1990年 | 1211篇 |
1989年 | 1139篇 |
1988年 | 1143篇 |
1987年 | 1093篇 |
1986年 | 1131篇 |
1985年 | 1230篇 |
1984年 | 952篇 |
1983年 | 900篇 |
1982年 | 686篇 |
1981年 | 654篇 |
1979年 | 1112篇 |
1978年 | 751篇 |
1977年 | 681篇 |
1976年 | 716篇 |
1975年 | 934篇 |
1974年 | 1065篇 |
1973年 | 1129篇 |
1972年 | 953篇 |
1971年 | 876篇 |
1970年 | 878篇 |
1969年 | 894篇 |
1968年 | 1141篇 |
1967年 | 1013篇 |
1966年 | 934篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
44.
45.
S Sipe R W Engle 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1986,12(3):402-412
Four experiments were conducted to determine whether echoic memory plays a role in differences between good and poor readers. The first two experiments used a suffix procedure in which the subject is read a list of digits with either a tone control or the word go appended to the list. For lists that exceeded the length of the subjects memory span by one digit (i.e., that avoided ceiling effects), the poor readers showed a larger decrement in the suffix condition than did the good readers. The third experiment was directed at the question of whether the duration of echoic memory is different for good and poor readers. Children shadowed words presented to one ear at a rate determined to give 75-85% shadowing accuracy. The items presented to the nonattended ear were words and an occasional digit. At various intervals after the presentation of the digit, a light signaled that the subject was to cease shadowing and attempt to recall any digit that had occurred in the nonattended ear recently. Whereas good and poor readers recalled the digit equally if tested immediately after presentation, the poor readers showed a faster decline in recall of the digit as retention interval increased. A fourth experiment was conducted to determine whether the differences in echoic memory were specific to speech stimuli or occurred at a more basic level of aural persistence. Bursts of white noise were separated by 9-400 ms of silence and the subject was to say whether there were one or two sounds presented. There were no differences in detectability functions for good and poor readers. 相似文献
46.
47.
48.
Collin L. Davidson MS LaRicka R. Wingate PhD Kathy A. Rasmussen BA Meredith L. Slish BA 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2009,39(5):499-507
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice. 相似文献
49.
Following Altman's privacy regulation model, this research was based on the assumption that the relationship between achieved and desired levels of privacy is a major determinant of the experience of crowding in small groups. Further, on the basis of evidence that advancing age is associated with a chronic lack of communicative interaction, it was assumed that the achieved level of privacy among elderly persons generally is greater than that experienced by young adults. Given these assumptions, it was predicted that the crowding threshold of young adults would be significantly lower than that of older persons. To test this hypothesis, the research varied conversation distance (close, far) and density (small room, large room) and assessed the quality of communicative interaction and task performance among small groups of college-age females and retired women over 60 years of age. Results showed that task performance among younger subjects deteriorated in close interaction conditions, whereas the performance of older subjects was improved by spatial intrusion. Further, older subjects exhibited positive communication behaviors in response to close conversation, high-density conditions, while younger subjects responded either with negative communication behaviors or were unaffected by spatial restriction. Finally, the research confirmed previous findings that conversation distance is a more important determinant of the experience of crowding in small groups than physical density. The significance of the results for Altman's privacy regulation model and their implications for the study of human communication behaviors were discussed. 相似文献
50.
Stephen R. Munzer 《The Journal of religious ethics》2005,33(4):747-781
Self‐abandonment and self‐denial are, respectively, Catholic and hyper‐Calvinist analogues of each other. Roughly, each requires the surrendering of a person to God's will and providence through faith, hope, and love. Should the self‐abandoning/self‐denying individual accept his or her own damnation if that be God's will? This article, which is virtually alone in discussing the Catholic and Reformed Protestant traditions together, answers “No.” The unqualified self‐abandonment present in quietism and the radical self‐denial of Samuel Hopkins are perverse and irrational responses to the prospect of hell because they run counter to the Christian's deepest need to spend eternity with God. However, a qualified self‐abandonment is intellectually defensible and offers a viable Christian piety. 相似文献