首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   65126篇
  免费   2760篇
  国内免费   37篇
  2020年   665篇
  2019年   795篇
  2018年   1114篇
  2017年   1091篇
  2016年   1234篇
  2015年   880篇
  2014年   1028篇
  2013年   4782篇
  2012年   1970篇
  2011年   1943篇
  2010年   1197篇
  2009年   1223篇
  2008年   1690篇
  2007年   1682篇
  2006年   1489篇
  2005年   1367篇
  2004年   1306篇
  2003年   1187篇
  2002年   1220篇
  2001年   2123篇
  2000年   2050篇
  1999年   1505篇
  1998年   698篇
  1997年   615篇
  1996年   591篇
  1992年   1348篇
  1991年   1254篇
  1990年   1256篇
  1989年   1149篇
  1988年   1137篇
  1987年   1069篇
  1986年   1093篇
  1985年   1205篇
  1984年   951篇
  1983年   851篇
  1982年   646篇
  1979年   1067篇
  1978年   720篇
  1977年   681篇
  1976年   691篇
  1975年   877篇
  1974年   1014篇
  1973年   1089篇
  1972年   923篇
  1971年   848篇
  1970年   818篇
  1969年   829篇
  1968年   1077篇
  1967年   931篇
  1966年   859篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Book Notes     
The Psychological Record -  相似文献   
992.
An examination of the social perception literature yields little evidence for the false-uniqueness phenomenon (Valins & Nisbett, 1972), the perception that one's attributes are more unique than is the case. In contrast, the tendency for individuals to project their own characteristics onto other people and assume that more people are like themselves is a robust phenomenon. One reason researchers may not have found false uniqueness is that they have not looked at the accuracy of consensus estimates. A close look at the results of Tabachnik, Crocker, and Alloy (1983) and Sanders and Mullen (1983), who did assess accuracy, suggests that people possessing undesirable attributes over-estimate consensus, whereas people holding desirable attributes underestimate consensus. The latter pattern is a form of false uniqueness. In this study we looked at the accuracy of social consensus estimates in the context of psychological fears. A sample of subjects filled out an abbreviated version of a fear survey and made estimates of consensus. The results showed that both high- and low-fear respondents overestimated the incidence of high fear among their peers, but high-fear subjects were more inaccurate in their estimates. A false-uniqueness effect was found on the part of low-fear subjects, as they tended to underestimate the incidence of low fear among their peers. These findings are consistent with a motivational interpretation that emphasizes the individual's need to justify or normalize stigmatized behavior and to bolster perceived self-competence.  相似文献   
993.
A conditional approach to dispositions is developed in which dispositional constructs are viewed as clusters of if-then propositions. These propositions summarize contingencies between categories of conditions and categories of behavior rather than generalized response tendencies. A fundamental unit for investigating dispositions is therefore the conditional frequency of acts that are central to a given behavior category in circumscribed situations, not the overall frequency of behaviors. In an empirical application of the model, we examine how people's dispositional judgments are linked to extensive observations of targets' behavior in a range of natural social situations. We identify categories of these social situations in which targets' behavior may be best predicted from observers' dispositional judgements, focusing on the domains of aggression and withdrawal. One such category consists of subjectively demanding or stressful situations that tax people's performance competencies. As expected, children judged to be aggressive or withdrawn were variable across situations in dispositionally relevant behaviors, but they diverged into relatively predictable aggressive and withdrawn actions in situations that required the social, self-regulatory, and cognitive competencies they lacked. Implications of the conditional approach for personality assessment and person perception research are considered.  相似文献   
994.
Intellectual functioning and aggression   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
In a 22-year study, data were collected on aggressiveness and intellectual functioning in more than 600 subjects, their parents, and their children. Both aggression and intellectual functioning are reasonably stable in a subject's lifetime and perpetuate themselves across generations and within marriage pairs. Aggression in childhood was shown to interfere with the development of intellectual functioning and to be predictive of poorer intellectual achievement as an adult. Early IQ was related to early subject aggression but did not predict changes in aggression after age 8. On the other hand, differences between early IQ and intellectual achievement in middle adulthood were predictable from early aggressive behavior. A dual-process model was offered to explain the relation between intellectual functioning and aggressive behavior. We hypothesized that low intelligence makes the learning of aggressive responses more likely at an early age, and this aggressive behavior makes continued intellectual development more difficult.  相似文献   
995.
996.
997.
998.
Optimal timing and the Weber function   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
999.
A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction   总被引:64,自引:0,他引:64  
  相似文献   
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号