全文获取类型
收费全文 | 86813篇 |
免费 | 1653篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
88474篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 1032篇 |
2019年 | 1279篇 |
2018年 | 1513篇 |
2017年 | 1510篇 |
2016年 | 1827篇 |
2015年 | 1350篇 |
2014年 | 1398篇 |
2013年 | 6689篇 |
2012年 | 2530篇 |
2011年 | 2662篇 |
2010年 | 1640篇 |
2009年 | 1595篇 |
2008年 | 2415篇 |
2007年 | 2388篇 |
2006年 | 2080篇 |
2005年 | 1936篇 |
2004年 | 1862篇 |
2003年 | 1707篇 |
2002年 | 1759篇 |
2001年 | 2693篇 |
2000年 | 2532篇 |
1999年 | 1949篇 |
1998年 | 991篇 |
1997年 | 869篇 |
1996年 | 808篇 |
1993年 | 788篇 |
1992年 | 1691篇 |
1991年 | 1567篇 |
1990年 | 1581篇 |
1989年 | 1397篇 |
1988年 | 1361篇 |
1987年 | 1333篇 |
1986年 | 1375篇 |
1985年 | 1480篇 |
1984年 | 1160篇 |
1983年 | 1046篇 |
1979年 | 1203篇 |
1978年 | 855篇 |
1977年 | 778篇 |
1976年 | 807篇 |
1975年 | 1077篇 |
1974年 | 1163篇 |
1973年 | 1195篇 |
1972年 | 1068篇 |
1971年 | 972篇 |
1970年 | 929篇 |
1969年 | 906篇 |
1968年 | 1178篇 |
1967年 | 1097篇 |
1966年 | 978篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
72.
LISE M. SAARI TERRY R. JOHNSON STEVEN D. MCLAUGHLIN DENISE M. ZIMMERLE 《Personnel Psychology》1988,41(4):731-743
A survey was systematically developed to assess issues related to management training and education, and it was sent to 1,000 randomly selected U.S. companies with at least 1,000 employees. Based on a 61% response rate, findings were obtained regarding issues such as needs assessment, management training and development approaches, reasons for selecting particular programs, characteristics of participants, how decisions are made regarding who will participate, preparation and follow-up of participants, evaluation of management training programs, future management training trends, and needed training content. Significant findings by company size and industry type also are reported. 相似文献
73.
This paper gives a method for determining a sample size that will achieve a prespecified bound on confidence interval width for the interrater agreement measure,. The same results can be used when a prespecified power is desired for testing hypotheses about the value of kappa. An example from the literature is used to illustrate the methods proposed here. 相似文献
74.
Renato A. Lewin 《Studia Logica》1988,47(4):387-389
We prove that there are two involutions defined by monadic terms that characterize Monadic Algebras. We further prove that the variety of Monadic Algebras is the smallest variety of Interior Algebras where these involutions give rise to an interpretation from the variety of Bounded Distributive Lattices into it.Research funded by Fondecyt, project N 764-1987. 相似文献
75.
We obtain in this paper a representation of the formulae of extensions ofL
by generalized quantifiers through functors between categories of first-order structures and partial isomorphisms. The main tool in the proofs is the back-and-forth technique. As a corollary we obtain the Caicedo's version of Fraïssés theorem characterizing elementary equivalence for such languages. We also discuss informally some geometrical interpretations of our results. 相似文献
76.
77.
M R Lamb 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》1988,14(1):96-104
Pigeons in a matching-to-sample experiment received (a) element samples (one of two colors or vertical or horizontal white lines), (b) separated compound samples (a color and a line element presented together), and (c) unified compound samples (vertical or horizontal colored lines). In Phase I, test stimuli were the two elements from one dimension; the dimension tested varied randomly when the sample was a compound. Element samples were matched better than unified samples, which were matched better than separated samples. In Phase II, two samples preceded the tests. On EC trials, the first sample (S1) was an element, and the second sample (S2) was a compound; on CE trials this order was reversed. One element of the compound sample was always the one appearing as the element sample on that trial (e.g., S1 = red, S2 = red-vertical). This element also served as the correct test alternative (e.g., test = red+ vs. green) except on probe trials when the correct test was the other element appearing in the compound (e.g., test = vertical+ vs. horizontal). Accuracy varied as a function of sample sequence when the compound was separated (EC greater than CE on nonprobe trials and CE greater than EC on prove trials). This pattern was not observed when the compound was unified. The data suggest that the element in EC trials served as a cue that increased processing of the cued dimension and decreased processing of the noncued dimension of separated compounds. However, both dimensions of unified compounds seem to have been processed regardless of cuing.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS) 相似文献
78.
Fighting pairs of isolated DBA/2 mice showed a significant increase in tail-flick response latencies independent of whether opponents were losing or winning the combat. The effect lasted less than 10 min in both animals. Elevated pain thresholds were also found in isolates that attacked a nonaggressive conspecific, and were prevented by naltrexone (0.2 mg/kg), while a larger dose (1.0 mg/kg) inhibited the attack behavior. A small increase in pain threshold was observed after exposure of isolates to the test box alone, while isolation per se had no effect on baseline tail-flick latencies. The data demonstrate that endogenous pain suppressing systems are activated during attack and suggest that this opioid-mediated antinociception is a correlate of the isolation syndrome, reflecting enhanced arousal of the attacking animal. 相似文献
79.
M R D'Amato M Colombo 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》1988,14(2):131-139
Cebus monkeys were trained on a five-item serial learning task, symbolized as ABCDE; the initial stages of training were on the shorter subseries AB, ABC, and ABCD. To assess the monkeys' knowledge of the sequential position of each item, pair-wise tests were given to 2 subjects after acquisition of the ABCD series and to 4 subjects after reaching criterion on the ABCDE series. In both tests, the monkeys performed at high levels on the interior pairs, which were BC for the ABCD series, and BC, BD, and CD for the ABCDE series. These results, as well as the orderly relations observed in the pair-wise tests between first-item response latency and first-item position and between second-item response latency and number of missing items, indicated that the monkeys had developed a well-organized internal representation of the four- and five-item series. Although pigeons are also capable of learning four-item and five-item series, they apparently do not develop a comparable representational structure. The disparity between the monkeys' and pigeons' representational competence for serial order is predictable from the difference in their capacities for associative transitivity. 相似文献
80.
The purpose of the present study was to determine whether the sex role orientations of male and female collegiate athletes were more similar in team sports than in individual sports. It was predicted that females in masculine-oriented team sports (basketball and volleyball) would exhibit sex role orientations more similar to those of their male counterparts than would females in individual sports (track/field and swimming). To test this notion the S. L. Bem (The Measurement of Psychological Androgyny, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1974, 42, 155–167) Sex Role Inventory was administered to 381 athletes from 37 National Collegiate Athletic Association institutions. The median-split procedure (S. L. Bem, On the Utility of Alternative Procedures for Assessing Psychology Androgyny, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1977, 45, 196–205) was used to classify subjects as masculine, feminine, androgynous, or undifferentiated. Separate Gender × Sex Role Orientation chi square tests were performed on the frequency data of team and individual sport athletes. The results indicated no significant differences in the sex role orientations of male and female team sport performers, with the greatest proportion (66%) having a masculine (33%) or androgynous (33%) orientation. For individual sports there was a significant difference in the sex role orientations of males and females. The highest proportion of females were classified as feminine (37%) and the lowest proportion were classified as masculine (11%). Individual sport males were somewhat evenly distributed among the four sex role orientation categories, with the highest proportion classified as undifferentiated (36%). It was concluded that sex role orientation of elite female athletes may be associated with the male appropriateness of the sport in which they participate. 相似文献