全文获取类型
收费全文 | 86777篇 |
免费 | 1652篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
88437篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 1032篇 |
2019年 | 1279篇 |
2018年 | 1511篇 |
2017年 | 1510篇 |
2016年 | 1830篇 |
2015年 | 1350篇 |
2014年 | 1397篇 |
2013年 | 6686篇 |
2012年 | 2531篇 |
2011年 | 2662篇 |
2010年 | 1641篇 |
2009年 | 1596篇 |
2008年 | 2415篇 |
2007年 | 2386篇 |
2006年 | 2077篇 |
2005年 | 1934篇 |
2004年 | 1862篇 |
2003年 | 1705篇 |
2002年 | 1752篇 |
2001年 | 2692篇 |
2000年 | 2532篇 |
1999年 | 1944篇 |
1998年 | 991篇 |
1997年 | 868篇 |
1996年 | 808篇 |
1993年 | 788篇 |
1992年 | 1693篇 |
1991年 | 1566篇 |
1990年 | 1581篇 |
1989年 | 1396篇 |
1988年 | 1359篇 |
1987年 | 1334篇 |
1986年 | 1374篇 |
1985年 | 1481篇 |
1984年 | 1159篇 |
1983年 | 1045篇 |
1979年 | 1202篇 |
1978年 | 854篇 |
1977年 | 776篇 |
1976年 | 807篇 |
1975年 | 1077篇 |
1974年 | 1163篇 |
1973年 | 1195篇 |
1972年 | 1068篇 |
1971年 | 972篇 |
1970年 | 925篇 |
1969年 | 905篇 |
1968年 | 1177篇 |
1967年 | 1096篇 |
1966年 | 978篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
321.
Alternation behavior, spatial discrimination, and reversal disturbances following 6-hydroxydopamine lesions in the nucleus accumbens of the rat 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
K Taghzouti A Louilot J P Herman M Le Moal H Simon 《Behavioral and neural biology》1985,44(3):354-363
The effects of dopaminergic depletion of the nucleus accumbens was tested in various behavioral tasks such as alternation, spatial discrimination, and reversal learning, and in an extinction paradigm in a T maze. Animals with lesions showed impairment of spontaneous alternation behavior, disturbances in the acquisition of spatial discrimination, and great difficulty in reversing previously learned habits. In the extinction phase, experimental animals are unable to adjust their behavior, and continue to choose the previously reinforced arm of the T maze. It is suggested that the nucleus accumbens plays an important role in the transition of motivation into action, and that dopamine has a facilitatory influence on the mediation of these processes. 相似文献
322.
To test the generality, across modality and across species, of a bidirectional effect of photic stimulation in infant rats found in previous experiments, two experiments examined the effects on heart rate (HR) of onset and offset of auditory stimulation in rats and of photic stimulation in guinea pigs. Experiment 1 demonstrated a unidirectional, decelerative response in rats 17 and 21 days of age to the onset and offset of a 1-kHz tone, at both low (80 dB) and high (100 dB) intensity levels and at 14 days to the onset of the high-intensity tone. Experiment 2 demonstrated that infant guinea pigs, which unlike infant rats are precocial and not nocturnal, responded to the offset but not to the onset of light, and that this response was decelerative. These results indicated a lack of generality of the bidirectional HR effect, and were discussed in terms of the orienting/defensive reflex distinction. 相似文献
323.
Bornstein’s hypothesis that there is a loss of color discrimination in tropical areas was not supported in earlier research
in the Cook Islands. Using a more sensitive color vision test (the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue Test), a loss of color discrimination
in the red-purple and blue-green regions of the spectrum was found. The lack of sex differences in this color discrimination
loss supports a physiological rather than a genetic explanation of the observed decrement. 相似文献
324.
This essay provides guidelines for designing a doctoral program in behavior analysis. First, we propose a general accomplishment for all behavior analytic doctoral students: that they be able to solve problems concerning individual behavior within a range of environments. Second, in order to achieve this goal, we propose that students be trained in conceptual and experimental analysis of behavior, the application of behavioral principles and the administration of behavioral programs. This training should include class work, but it should emphasize the immersion of students in a variety of environments in which they are required to use behavior analytic strategies. Third, we provide an example of a hypothetical graduate program that involves the proposed training. Finally, an evaluation plan is suggested for determining whether a training program is in fact producing students who are generalized problem-solvers. At each step, we justify our point of view from a perspective that combines principles from behavior analysis and educational systems design. 相似文献
325.
326.
Paul C. Quinn B. R. Wooten Evette J. Ludman 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1985,37(3):198-204
Sternheim and Boynton’s (1966) continuous judgmental color-naming technique was used to assess the elemental nature of achromatic colors. Results from three subjects indicate that the names “black” and “white” are necessary and sufficient to describe achromatic test lights associated with the name “gray.” On the basis of the criteria that establishes a color as elemental, black and white were confirmed as elemental, whereas gray was not. These findings support the assumption of various opponent processing models that there are only two elemental achromatic sensations. In addition, the results support Hering’s (1920) view that black and white differ from the paired-chromatic opponent colors in that they are not mutually exclusive. 相似文献
327.
Carl E. Granrud Albert Yonas Elizabeth A. Opland 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1985,37(5):415-419
Five- and 7-month-old infants were tested for sensitivity to the depth cue of shading. Infants were presented with two displays: a surface in which a convexity and a concavity were molded and a photograph in which shading specified a convexity and a concavity. Each display was presented under both monocular and binocular viewing conditions. Reaching was observed as the dependent measure. Infants in both age groups reached preferentially for the actual convexity in both the monocular and binocular viewing conditions. In the monocular photograph condition, the 7-month-olds reached preferentially for the apparent convexity specified by shading, indicating that they perceived it to be an actual convexity. These infants showed no significant reaching preference in the binocular photograph condition. This finding rules out interpretations of the infants’ reaching not based on perceived depth. The results therefore suggest that the 7-month-olds perceived depth from shading. The 5-month-olds showed no significant reaching preferences when viewing the photograph; thus, they showed no evidence of depth perception from shading. These findings are consistent with the results of a number of studies that have investigated infants’ sensitivity to pictorial depth cues. Together, these studies suggest that the ability to perceive depth from pictorial cues may first develop between 5 and 7 months of age. 相似文献
328.
329.
330.
This study concerns the manner in which the traumatic events suffered by the parent in the Nazi holocaust are communicated to the second generation born after the Second World War. Additionally we investigated the effects of differences in wartime experiences on subsequent communication behaviors. Communication behaviors in families whose parents had been prisoners in concentration camps were compared with those of families who had displayed active resistance (i.e., were partisans) during the Second World War. The data indicate greater legitimacy and openness in discussing holocaust-related issues in the homes of ex-partisans than in the homes of ex-prisoners in concentration camps. Also, sons and daughters of the former group have better knowledge of the holocaust and hold more favorable attitudes than do sons and daughters in the second group. The implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献