全文获取类型
收费全文 | 11392篇 |
免费 | 266篇 |
国内免费 | 43篇 |
专业分类
11701篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 1164篇 |
2011年 | 1350篇 |
2010年 | 310篇 |
2009年 | 115篇 |
2008年 | 1009篇 |
2007年 | 1066篇 |
2006年 | 946篇 |
2005年 | 788篇 |
2004年 | 726篇 |
2003年 | 767篇 |
2002年 | 659篇 |
2001年 | 460篇 |
2000年 | 533篇 |
1999年 | 251篇 |
1998年 | 61篇 |
1997年 | 53篇 |
1996年 | 47篇 |
1995年 | 35篇 |
1994年 | 37篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 37篇 |
1991年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 32篇 |
1988年 | 27篇 |
1987年 | 36篇 |
1985年 | 27篇 |
1984年 | 30篇 |
1983年 | 46篇 |
1982年 | 32篇 |
1981年 | 25篇 |
1980年 | 23篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 23篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 27篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1965年 | 16篇 |
1959年 | 31篇 |
1958年 | 97篇 |
1957年 | 69篇 |
1956年 | 73篇 |
1955年 | 65篇 |
1954年 | 59篇 |
1953年 | 52篇 |
1952年 | 46篇 |
1951年 | 51篇 |
1950年 | 48篇 |
1949年 | 25篇 |
1948年 | 27篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Pigeons' not pecking or pecking constituted choice between a delayed, large reinforcer and an immediate, small reinforcer (self-control) and at other times between a delayed reinforcer and no reinforcer (omission). Both a tone and a keylight were tested as choice signals, and the delayed reinforcer was either response independent or response dependent. Pigeons pecked during the choice signals on over 95% of the trials in the self-control procedure, and pecked during the choice signals on over 75% of the trials in the omission procedure. Consistent pecking was observed with either the tone or the keylight as a choice signal, with the exception that a tone paired with a response-independent delayed reinforcer did not maintain pecking in the omission procedure. Pigeons pecked during more choice signals when delayed reinforcers were response dependent than when the delayed reinforcers were response independent. These results indicate that Pavlovian conditioning influences self-control experiments, especially in single-key procedures. 相似文献
12.
Persistence and its assessment have been studied since the 1920s. One common approach in measuring persistence has been through testing the subject's ability to solve difficult or monotonous problems. Another approach has been through questionnaires. Although persistence is very important in academic achievement and in the general development of personality, no questionnaires have been developed for measuring persistence in children. The present research concerns the development of such a scale. It includes 40 items and was tested on 322 Israeli children aged 7-13. The internal reliability of the scale (Cronbach alpha) was .66, and the test-retest reliability after 6 months was .77. The scale differentiated active gymnasts from non-gymnasts as well as persistent gymnasts from dropout gymnasts. In addition, significant correlations were found between the scale and the need-persistence measure of the Rosenzweig Picture Frustration Study, Locus of Control, and other measures. We discuss these findings along with the significance of persistence in everyday life. 相似文献
13.
Faust D 《Journal of personality assessment》1986,50(4):585-600
A meeting of Psychologists Against Tough-Hearted and Offensive Scientists (PATHOS) was convened to redress the damage done by judgment researchers who have degraded the art of clinical decision making. A special task force drafted a rebuttal and mission statement, which is presented in its entirety. 相似文献
14.
It is suggested that molar streams of behavior are constructed of various arrangements of three elementary constituents (elicited, evoked, and emitted response classes). An eight-cell taxonomy is elaborated as a framework for analyzing and synthesizing complex behavioral repertoires based on these functional units. It is proposed that the local force binding functional units into a smoothly articulated kinetic sequence arises from temporally arranged relative response probability relationships. Behavioral integration is thought to reflect the joint influence of the organism's hierarchy of relative response probabilities, fluctuating biological states, and the arrangement of environmental and behavioral events in time. 相似文献
15.
Epstein R 《The Behavior analyst / MABA》1984,7(2):101-119
Since the early 1900s a variety of names has been proposed for the scientific study of behavior, but none has come into general use. "Praxics," a recent entry, is defensible on several grounds. "Behaviorism," on the other hand, is the name of a school of philosophy. Though praxics has roots in behaviorism, the term "behaviorism" should not be applied to praxics. Confusion between the science and the philosophy has retarded the growth of the science immeasurably. Its growth has also been impeded by its association with psychology, which is still primarily the study of mind. Efforts are underway to establish praxics as an independent field. 相似文献
16.
Poppen R 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1972,18(1):119-127
Young adults performed a lever-pressing task for money on two schedules of reinforcement: concurrent fixed-interval 1 min—differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate 20-sec, and concurrent fixed-interval 1-min—fixed ratio 100 responses. All subjects were trained on both schedules. Fixed-interval performance concurrent with the differential reinforcement procedure was characterized by high constant rates with no post-reinforcement pauses. Fixed-interval performance concurrent with fixed ratio was characterized by low rates and lengthy post-reinforcement pauses. These results differ from those obtained in prior studies on the effects of conditioning history upon subsequent fixed-interval performance. The prior work, using non-concurrent procedures, had shown that fixed-interval performance following differential reinforcement of low rates was characterized by post-reinforcement pauses and low rates, while fixed-interval performance following fixed ratio exhibited high constant rates and no post-reinforcement pause. The present results suggest that alternative concurrent contingencies are another major determinant of human fixed-interval performance. 相似文献
17.
In two experiments, the effect of an illuminated response key on the acquisition of stimulus control by an airflow stimulus was assessed. In the first experiment, pigeons were given nondifferential training with airflow emerging from behind the response key in one of three conditions of illumination: trained to peck a lighted key, trained to peck an unlighted key with a houselight present, trained to peck a key in total darkness. After 10 days of training on a variable-interval schedule of reinforcement, all subjects were given a generalization test on airflow velocity. The gradients for subjects trained in the dark were sharp, while those for subjects trained in lighted conditions were shallow. In the second experiment, the effect of an irrelevant keylight on the acquisition of an airflow velocity discrimination was assessed. Two groups of pigeons were trained to discriminate two airflow velocities. One group was trained with a lighted response key and the other was trained to peck the response key in total darkness. The dark-trained subjects acquired the discrimination more rapidly. The results demonstrate that the acquisition of stimulus control by airflow with either a differential or nondifferential training procedure can be overshadowed by keylight. 相似文献
18.
19.
Coleman R 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1970,3(4):293-297
A procedure was developed for use in a public school classroom where only one of the children needed treatment, sophisticated apparatus was not feasible, personnel were untrained in conditioning techniques, and where disruption had to be minimized. Candy reinforcers were contingent upon working behavior. The total candy earned in the session was divided equally among the class. Portable radio control apparatus was adopted to give feedback to the child when he was displaying the desired working behavior. An increase in working behavior and a decrease in talking aloud and out-of-seat behavior was observed for each of the four subjects. When the feedback apparatus was removed, the desired behavior was maintained through candy reinforcement alone in all four subjects. 相似文献
20.