首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   273篇
  免费   16篇
  2023年   2篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   11篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   8篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   8篇
  2013年   25篇
  2012年   12篇
  2011年   11篇
  2010年   12篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   10篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   14篇
  2005年   7篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   9篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   4篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   7篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   4篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   2篇
  1978年   2篇
  1974年   1篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   2篇
  1966年   1篇
  1960年   1篇
  1958年   1篇
  1951年   2篇
  1950年   1篇
  1947年   1篇
  1942年   1篇
  1941年   1篇
排序方式: 共有289条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
261.
Sun YH  Ge L  Quinn PC  Wang Z  Xiao NG  Pascalis O  Tanaka J  Lee K 《Perception》2012,41(1):117-120
We report a novel fat face illusion that when two identical images of the same face are aligned vertically, the face at the bottom appears 'fatter'. This illusion emerged when the faces were shown upright, but not inverted, with the size of the illusion being 4%. When the faces were presented upside down, the illusion did not emerge. Also, when upright clocks were shown in the same vertically aligned fashion, we did not observe the illusion, indicating that the fat illusion does not generalize to every category of canonically upright objects with similar geometric shape as a face.  相似文献   
262.
263.
Early life stress (ELS) experiences can cause changes in cognitive and affective functioning. This study examined the persistent effects of a single traumatic event in infancy on several adult behavioral outcomes in male and female C57BL/6J mice. Mice received 15 footshocks in infancy and were tested for stress-enhanced fear learning, extinction learning, discrimination and reversal learning, and novel object recognition. Infant trauma potentiated fear learning in adulthood and produced resistance to extinction but did not influence other behaviors, suggesting restricted effects of infant trauma on behaviors reliant on cortico-amygdala circuitry.

Exposure to traumatic events early in childhood is associated with the development of psychiatric disorders (Copeland et al. 2018) and deficits in cognitive and affective functioning (Pechtel and Pizzagalli 2011) in adulthood. In humans, early life stress (ELS) is defined as experiencing traumatic events in childhood (Pechtel and Pizzagalli 2011). Rodent models of ELS suggest that acute versus chronic stress may differentially alter systems that regulate the stress response, producing different behavioral outcomes in adulthood (Pryce et al. 2002; Musazzi et al. 2017).Stress-enhanced fear learning (SEFL), a powerful preclinical model of PTSD- and addiction-like behaviors, captures the enduring, maladaptive effects of a single traumatic event on behavior (Rau et al. 2009; Meyer et al. 2013; Radke et al. 2019). In these studies, exposure to 15 footshocks enhances contextual fear conditioning later in life. For infant SEFL, enhanced contextual fear conditioning occurs months after the initial stressful experience and in the absence of memory for the context in which ELS was experienced (Poulos et al. 2014; Quinn et al. 2014). SEFL protocols have been used to model adult or infant trauma in rats (e.g., Rau et al. 2009; Poulos et al. 2014; Quinn et al. 2014) and have been extended to adult mice (Sillivan et al. 2017; Hassien et al. 2020; Pennington et al. 2020). However, to our knowledge, no studies have established the use of acute, infant footshock as a model of ELS in mice.We sought to characterize the effects of acute, infant trauma exposure across several types of learning in adult mice. We tested mice exposed to 15 footshocks on postnatal day (PND) 17 for contextual fear learning, extinction of fear, discrimination and reversal learning, and novel object recognition. Our results suggest that exposure to acute infant trauma enhances fear learning and resistance to extinction in adulthood, but does not alter other types of learning.Male and female C57BL/6J mice were generated from breeding pairs from The Jackson Laboratory. Mice were group-housed (two to four mice/cage) post-weaning and were provided food and water ad libitum, unless otherwise specified. Mice were on a 12:12 light/ dark cycle. Other than trauma exposure, all behavioral tests were conducted during adulthood (PND 60+). Animals were cared for in accordance with the guidelines set by the National Institutes of Health and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Miami University.We first established that exposure to infant footshock produces enhanced contextual fear conditioning in adulthood. Mice were placed in a MED-Associates conditioning chamber (context A) on PND 17 (Fig. 1A; after Quinn et al. 2014). Context A was brightly lit, contained a uniform grid floor, was scented with vanilla (50%), and was cleaned with odorless 5% sodium hydroxide. Mice received either 0 or 15 footshocks (1 mA, 1 sec) during a 60-min session beginning 180 sec following placement in the chamber. Progressive scan video cameras containing visible light filters monitored mice throughout the session. Video Freeze software (Med Associates, Inc.) analyzed the video and data were expressed as percent of time spent freezing during the session. Fear conditioning experiments were powered to detect sex differences with an n of eight per sex. Because differences were not observed when sex was included as a factor in analyses, all reported results represent data from both sexes. Due to computer malfunction, fear conditioning sessions from 14 mice were hand scored according to standard time-sampling procedures (Chowdhury et al. 2005) and data from three mice had to be excluded on extinction session 2.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Acute infant trauma produces stress-enhanced fear learning in adulthood. (A) Experimental timeline and visual representation of context A and context B. Infant trauma consisted of 15 footshocks or no footshocks on PND 17 in context A. Adult fear conditioning consisted of one footshock (black bars) or no footshock (blue bars) on PND 60 in context B. Extinction was assessed in context B. Memory of the infant trauma was assessed in context A. Stress enhanced fear learning (SEFL) was observed in mice who were exposed to early life stress (15 shocks) and fear conditioning (one shock) for extinction test 1 (B) and extinction test 2 (C). (*) P < 0.05, (**) P < 0.01 versus no/one shock group (Holm–Sidak test). (D) Mice showed little freezing, indicating an absence of fear memory for the infant trauma in context A. Data are means ± SEM.Contextual fear conditioning occurred on PND 60 in a novel context (context B). Context B was dark with a staggered grid floor and cleaned and scented with acetic acid (5%). Baseline freezing during the first 180 sec in this novel context was assessed and used to measure generalization between the stress exposure context (context A) and the novel fear conditioning context (context B). Mice received either 0 or 1 footshocks (1 mA, 1 sec) 180 sec into a 3.5-min session. Thus, there were four groups (infant trauma/adult fear conditioning): no/no shock, n = 14; 15/no shock, n = 15; no/one shock, n = 15; and 15/one shock, n = 18. To test extinction of fear memory, mice were reintroduced to context B for two 8-min sessions, separated by 24 h. Finally, mice were reintroduced to context A for an 8-min retention test of the original context.Mice exposed to 15 footshocks in infancy and conditioned with one footshock as adults exhibited robust SEFL. There were no differences in baseline freezing during the adult fear conditioning session (infant trauma: F(1,47) = 2.67, P = 0.109, no/no shocks = 0.26 ± 0.15; no/one shock = 2.17 ± 0.64; 15/no shocks = 2.14 ± 0.76; 15/one shock = 7.17 ± 3.25; all data mean ± SEM). For extinction sessions (Fig. 1B,C), a mixed-effects analysis identified main effects of trauma (F(1,58) = 5.68, P = 0.020), fear conditioning (F(1,58) = 12.40, P < 0.001), and session (F(1,55) = 5.25, P = 0.026). There was a significant interaction between fear conditioning and session (F(1,55) = 7.10, P = 0.010). Two-way ANOVA was next used to examine each test session. For session 1 (Fig. 1B), there were main effects of trauma (F(1,58) = 5.72, P = 0.020) and fear conditioning (F(1,58) = 21.10, P < 0.001). The interaction of trauma and fear conditioning did not reach the threshold for significance (F(1,58) = 2.87, P = 0.096). Follow-up Holm Sidak''s tests revealed that adult fear conditioning produced greater freezing in mice exposed to infant footshock vs. trauma-naïve mice (P = 0.008). For the second extinction test (Fig. 1C), the main effects of trauma (F(1,55) = 3.72, P = 0.059) and fear conditioning (F(1,55) = 3.92, P = 0.053) approached the threshold for significance. Follow-up Holm Sidak''s tests revealed that trauma-exposed mice froze more than trauma-naïve mice following adult fear conditioning only (P = 0.042). When tested for memory of the context used for trauma-exposure on PND 17 (context A), mice demonstrated minimal freezing and two-way ANOVA found no significant main effects or interactions (Fs < 1.39) (Fig. 1D). Percent freezing in infancy and in extinction test 1 in adulthood were correlated (r = −0.481, P = 0.043) for the 15/one group alone. Activity bursts in infancy did not correlate with freezing behavior in adulthood. These results indicate that mice exposed to infant trauma who experienced fear conditioning in adulthood exhibited SEFL on the first and second extinction sessions but the memory of the trauma experience was not retained into adulthood.Since PTSD is associated with deficits in fear extinction (Zuj et al. 2016), we next examined extinction learning using 30-min sessions (Fig. 2A). Mice were exposed to acute infant trauma (0 or 15 footshocks) on PND 17. On PND 60, mice were reintroduced to context A for an 8-min test of memory for the original ELS context prior to fear conditioning (no/one shock = 2.41 ± 0.58; 15/one shock = 3.50 ± 0.42; no/three shocks = 1.86 ± 0.43). On PND 61 fear conditioning occurred in context B. Since we observed that trauma-naïve mice displayed very little fear conditioning following one footshock (Fig. 1B,C), mice in this experiment received either one or three footshocks during adult fear conditioning. There were three groups (infant trauma/adult fear conditioning): no/one shock, n = 17; 15/one shock, n = 17; no/three shocks, n = 18. Retention of fear memory was tested for five subsequent days (PND 62–66) in context B during 30-min sessions.Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Extinction of fear learning is impaired following acute infant trauma. (A) Experimental timeline. Infant trauma consisted of 15 footshocks or no footshocks on PND 17 in context A. Adult fear conditioning consisted of one (yellow symbols) or three (blue symbols) footshocks in trauma-naïve mice and one footshock (black symbols) in trauma-exposed mice in context B. Memory of the infant trauma was assessed in context A. Mice received five extinction sessions in context B. (B) On extinction day 1, trauma-exposed mice initially froze at the same level as trauma-naïve mice conditioned with three footshocks but fear persisted longer, demonstrating within-session resistance to extinction. (*) P < 0.05, (**) P < 0.01 15/one shock group versus no/one shock group; (#) P < 0.05 15/one shock group versus no/three shocks group; () P < 0.05, () P < 0.01 no/one shock group versus no/three shock group (Holm–Sidak test). (C) Freezing was averaged across the first 8 min of each extinction session. Trauma-exposed mice demonstrated resistance to extinction across sessions. On session 1, both trauma-exposed (15/one shock group) and mice conditioned with three footshocks (no/three shock group) had elevated freezing compared to mice conditioned with one footshock. On session 2, freezing in trauma-exposed mice remained elevated and was greater than in the other two groups. (**) P < 0.01 15/one shock group versus no/one shock group. (##) P < 0.01 15/one shock group versus no/three shocks group. () P < 0.05 no/one shock group versus no/three shocks group (Holm–Sidak test). Data are means ± SEM.On the first extinction session, there were significant main effects of time (F(29,1421) = 2.23, P < 0.001) and group (F(2,49) = 3.54, P = 0.037) and a significant interaction (F(58,1421) = 1.93, P < 0.001). Holm–Sidak follow-up comparisons revealed that trauma-exposed mice (15/one shock) and mice conditioned with three footshocks during adulthood (no/three shocks) froze more than the no/one shock group during the first 3 min (P < 0.05 for minute 1 and P < 0.01 for minutes 2 and 3) of the first extinction session (Fig. 2B). Trauma-exposed mice continued to freeze more than the no/one shock group during minutes 4–7 (P < 0.01) and minute 11 (P < 0.05). Freezing in the no/3 shocks group was similar to trauma-exposed mice for the first 4 min but diminished sooner, evidenced by a significant difference between these groups during minutes 5, 6, and 9 (P < 0.05). These results suggest that conditioning with three footshocks produces similar levels of fear in trauma-naïve mice as conditioning with one footshock in trauma-exposed mice, but that within-session extinction is delayed in the trauma-exposed group.To examine extinction across the five sessions, we averaged freezing during the first 8 min of each session. We found significant main effects of session (F(4,196) = 22.92, P < 0.001) and group (F(2,49) = 6.38, P = 0.003) and a significant interaction (F(8,196) = 2.75, P = 0.007). Holm–Sidak follow-up comparisons revealed that mice exposed to infant trauma (15/one shock) and mice in the no/three shock group froze more than those in the no/one shock group on extinction session 1 (P < 0.01 and P < 0.05, respectively) (Fig. 2C). On session 2, freezing was greater in trauma-exposed mice versus both other groups (P < 0.01). Percent freezing in infancy did not correlate with freezing behavior in adulthood. Activity bursts in infancy correlated with freezing behavior in adulthood on extinction test 1 (r = −0.558, P = 0.020). These results further suggest that infant trauma produces resistance to extinction.To determine whether the behavioral alterations observed in mice exposed to infant trauma extend to other types of learning, we tested the effects of infant footshock on operant discrimination and reversal learning for food reward and novel object recognition. For discrimination and reversal learning, we used a subset of mice from the first experiment (no shock = 20, 15 shocks = 17) restricted to 85% of free-feeding weight. Mice underwent one habituation day with ten 14-mg grain pellets (Bio Serv) in their home cages. The following day, mice began 15-min training sessions in a standard mouse operant chamber (Med Associates). There were two nose-poke holes and a reward receptacle on one wall of the chamber and a house light and speaker on the opposite wall. The chamber was housed in a sound and light-attenuating box and connected to a computer for data collection (Med-PC V software suite). For all sessions, the house light was off and lights in the nose-poke holes were on. Following a correct response, a 2-sec, 65-dB tone sounded and there was a timeout period of 20 sec following reward delivery during which the lights above the nose-poke holes were off and no rewards could be earned.During the first session, 30 pellets were automatically delivered into the reward receptacle. Next, mice were trained to respond for the food reward on a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) schedule by responding at either nose-poke hole until meeting criterion of 30 responses in 15-min. For discrimination, mice were trained to respond at the active nose-poke hole (100% probability of reward), which was randomly assigned to the left or right side. The contingencies of the active and inactive nose-pokes holes were reversed once criterion was met (≥30 rewards with 85% reinforced responses over two consecutive sessions).Neither sex nor adult fear conditioning affected any measure of discrimination and reversal learning, so all results are reported collapsed across these two factors. Two trauma-exposed females did not acquire the discrimination in 25 sessions and were not advanced to reversal. Data were analyzed using mixed-effects analyses with phase (i.e., acquisition and reversal) as the within-subjects factor. There were no differences between groups in the total number of sessions required to complete discrimination (no shock = 5.70 ± 0.69; 15 shock = 6.82 ± 1.50) or reversal (no shock = 9.40 ± 0.98; 15 shock = 7.73 ± 1.21). Mice made more total reinforced (main effect of phase: F(1,33) = 11.62, P = 0.002) and total nonreinforced (main effect of phase: F(1,33) = 42.54, P < 0.001) responses during reversal but there were no effects of infant trauma on behavior (Fig. 3A,B). These results indicate that acute infant trauma does not influence acquisition of operant discrimination learning or behavioral flexibility in adulthood.Open in a separate windowFigure 3.Acute infant trauma does not affect discrimination and reversal learning or novel object recognition. (A,B) Following infant trauma on PND 17 (no footshock, blue, or 15 footshocks, black), adult mice were trained to respond for a food pellet in an operant, spatial discrimination and reversal learning task. Following acquisition of the discrimination (left or right nose-poke hole reinforced 100% of the time) the contingencies of the responses were reversed. Mice made more total reinforced (A) and unreinforced (B) responses during reversal versus acquisition across sessions ([**] P < 0.01, main effect of training phase) but there were no effects of infant trauma exposure. (C) A separate cohort of mice exposed to infant trauma (no or 15 footshocks on PND 17) was tested for novel object recognition by assessing exploration of a novel object 1 and 24 h after exposure to the familiar object. Mice explored the novel object more ([**] P < 0.01, main effect of object, main effect of testing session, and interaction of object × testing session) but there were no effects of infant trauma exposure. Data are means ± SEM.In a new cohort of mice (no shock = 16, 15 shocks = 16), novel object recognition following infant footshock was tested. In adulthood, mice were handled for 1–3 min for two consecutive days. The following day, mice were placed in 20 × 18 × 25-cm apparatus with white floors and patterned walls (Panlab) for a 10-min habituation session to the chamber. Twenty-four hours later, mice were returned to the apparatus now containing two sample objects for 10-min initial exposure to the objects (two identical small plastic caps, 0.8 cm tall with a 2.1-cm diameter) placed in the back right and left corners of the apparatus. One hour later, mice were returned to the apparatus for a 3-min session (1-h test) where one of the sample objects was replaced with a novel object (a 2.5 × 1.2 × 1.5-cm Lego tower). The object replaced was alternated for each mouse. Mice were returned to the same box 24 h later for another 3-min session (24-h test) with the opposite cap replaced with a second novel object (a 3 × 1.5-cm black binder clip). ANY-maze software recorded each session. Time spent interacting with each object was measured by two independent raters and averaged (after Bevins and Besheer 2006).A three-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of object (familiar vs. novel; F(1,60) = 59.84, P < 0.001). There was also a main effect of testing session (1-h vs. 24-h test; F(1,60) = 7.89, P = 0.007) and an interaction of object × testing session (F(1,60) = 11.48, P = 0.001) (Fig. 3C,D). Interrater reliability was confirmed using Pearson''s correlation (r = 0.942). These results indicate that acute infant trauma does not influence hippocampal-dependent object recognition memory.Our results demonstrate that an acute traumatic experience during infancy affects some learned behaviors during adulthood. As previously reported in rats (Quinn et al. 2014; Poulos et al. 2014), 15 footshocks on PND 17 increased adult contextual fear conditioning in mice. We also demonstrated for the first time that the infant SEFL protocol produces resistance to extinction (within-session and between-session) and that behavior in a discrimination and reversal learning task and a novel object recognition task are unaffected. These findings establish the use of infant footshock to study SEFL in mice and further support the use of this paradigm as a model of PTSD-like behavior.The effects observed here differ from those commonly observed following chronic ELS manipulations such as limited nesting and bedding (Ivy et al. 2008; Molet et al. 2016) or maternal separation (Nishi et al. 2014). Chronic ELS impairs acquisition of fear conditioning in adult rodents (Kosten et al. 2006; Stevenson et al. 2009; Lesuis et al. 2019) and performance on hippocampal-dependent memory tasks (Rice et al. 2008; Naninck et al. 2015), for example. These behavioral differences suggest that acute and chronic infant stressors alter neural circuits in unique ways that are worthy of further study.Since the tasks used here rely on distinct neural circuits, the current results provide novel insight into how acute infant trauma impacts brain function. The effects of acute ELS were restricted to fear acquisition and extinction, suggesting alterations in cortico-amygdala circuits (Tovote et al. 2015). However, preservation of novel object recognition as well as discrimination and reversal learning suggest that hippocampal and striatal circuits likely remain intact (Cohen and Stackman 2015; Izquierdo et al. 2017). These findings can guide future studies concerning the neural mechanisms of acute ELS effects on behavior.  相似文献   
264.
Three‐ to 4‐month‐old infants show asymmetric exclusivity in the acquisition of cat and dog perceptual categories. The cat perceptual category excludes dog exemplars, but the dog perceptual category does not exclude cat exemplars. We describe a connectionist autoencoder model of perceptual categorization that shows the same asymmetries as infants. The model predicts the presence of asymmetric retroactive interference when infants acquire cat and dog categories sequentially. A subsequent experiment conducted with 3‐ to 4‐month‐olds verifies the predicted pattern of looking time behaviors. We argue that bottom‐up, associative learning systems with distributed representations are appropriate for modeling the operation of short‐term visual memory in early perceptual category learning.  相似文献   
265.
266.
267.
We explored how women talk about feminism and feminists and position themselves in relation to a feminist identity within a conversational setting. Nine pairs of female graduate and senior undergraduate students talked about feminism in sessions lasting 60 minutes. Sessions were analyzed using discourse analysis. Participants positioned themselves in multiple ways in relation to feminism. Notably, a feminist subject position was both difficult to take up and difficult to reject. In resolving the dilemma, to be or not to be feminist, participants drew on three interpretative repertoires: a liberal version emphasizing equality and rights, an extremist version emphasizing undesirable extremism, and a lifestyle feminist version entailing being feminist by virtue of how one lives. We discuss the implications of this multiplicity for conceptualizing feminist identity and for feminism as a political project.  相似文献   
268.
Debate continues about the accuracy of military suicide reporting due to concerns that some suicides may be classified as accidents to minimize stigma and ensure survivor benefits. We systematically reviewed records for 998 active duty Army deaths (510 suicides; 488 accident, homicide, and undetermined deaths; 2005‐2009) and, using research criteria, reclassified 8.2% of the nonsuicide cases to definite suicide (1), suicide probable (4), or suicide possible (35). The reclassification rate to definite suicide was only 0.2% (1/488). This low rate suggests that flagrant misclassification of Army deaths is uncommon and surveillance reports likely reflect the “true” population of Army suicides.  相似文献   
269.
270.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号