全文获取类型
收费全文 | 34963篇 |
免费 | 648篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
35617篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 446篇 |
2019年 | 530篇 |
2018年 | 754篇 |
2017年 | 727篇 |
2016年 | 811篇 |
2015年 | 515篇 |
2014年 | 635篇 |
2013年 | 2771篇 |
2012年 | 1216篇 |
2011年 | 1243篇 |
2010年 | 739篇 |
2009年 | 714篇 |
2008年 | 1036篇 |
2007年 | 992篇 |
2006年 | 907篇 |
2005年 | 743篇 |
2004年 | 705篇 |
2003年 | 640篇 |
2002年 | 663篇 |
2001年 | 1061篇 |
2000年 | 1086篇 |
1999年 | 790篇 |
1998年 | 336篇 |
1997年 | 311篇 |
1995年 | 312篇 |
1992年 | 633篇 |
1991年 | 596篇 |
1990年 | 575篇 |
1989年 | 554篇 |
1988年 | 545篇 |
1987年 | 486篇 |
1986年 | 481篇 |
1985年 | 533篇 |
1984年 | 437篇 |
1983年 | 403篇 |
1982年 | 310篇 |
1979年 | 476篇 |
1978年 | 332篇 |
1977年 | 306篇 |
1976年 | 320篇 |
1975年 | 362篇 |
1974年 | 439篇 |
1973年 | 468篇 |
1972年 | 401篇 |
1971年 | 344篇 |
1970年 | 322篇 |
1969年 | 338篇 |
1968年 | 422篇 |
1967年 | 371篇 |
1966年 | 360篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
281.
C. R. Ingils 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1968,46(8):744-748
In our society an increasingly greater emphasis is being placed on group interaction and decision-making. Persons participating in such a process often use the term “group dynamics” to describe what is thought to be a social-psychological phenomenon. This article discusses some of the elements of “true” group characteristics and effects, and their possible existence in “group dynamic” situations. The article also deals with the implications of the effects of these elements in “group dynamic” environments on educational and administrative leadership and decision-making. 相似文献
282.
283.
284.
Shimp CP 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1967,10(1):57-65
When a pigeon's choices between two keys are probabilistically reinforced, as in discrete trial probability learning procedures and in concurrent variable-interval schedules, the bird tends to maximize, or to choose the alternative with the higher probability of reinforcement. In concurrent variable-interval schedules, steady-state matching, which is an approximate equality between the relative frequency of a response and the relative frequency of reinforcement of that response, has previously been obtained only as a consequence of maximizing. In the present experiment, maximizing was impossible. A choice of one of two keys was reinforced only if it formed, together with the three preceding choices, the sequence of four successive choices that had occurred least often. This sequence was determined by a Bernoulli-trials process with parameter p. Each of three pigeons matched when p was ½ or ¼. Therefore, steady-state matching by individual birds is not always a consequence of maximizing. Choice probability varied between successive reinforcements, and sequential statistics revealed dependencies which were adequately described by a Bernoulli-trials process with p depending on the time since the preceding reinforcement. 相似文献
285.
C P Shimp 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1967,10(5):425-434
Five contingencies were superimposed successively on a variable-interval schedule of reinforcement. In each of the resulting conditions, a different short, interresponse time was reinforced and an interresponse-time distribution was obtained from each of three pigeons. The lower bound of the reinforced interresponse times ranged from 0.3 to 2.4 sec. The resulting distributions were combined, according to a rationale based upon concurrent operants, induction, and a property of variable-interval schedules, to describe the interresponse-time distributions from a variable-interval schedule. 相似文献
286.
287.
288.
Demand characteristics were studied in a simple situation in which observers made size and distance estimates of stimuli presented on a two-dimensional linear perspective drawing. Half the Ss were asked questions stressing phenomenal report: The other half were asked the same questions preceded by instructions stressing the objective nature of the stimulus configuration but requesting, nonetheless, phenomenal report. Instructions resulted in a significantly greater perspective size illusion but did not affect the distance estimates. No meaningful correlations between size and distance estimates were found. 相似文献
289.
Marilyn C. Smith 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1967,19(4):350-352
Proponents of a “single channel” theory of the psychological refractory period have not specified whether the single channel occupies only the decision component of the response selection, only the motor or response component, or both. In this experiment, the delay in the RT to the second of two successively presented stimuli was examined as a function of whether or not an overt motor response was made to the first stimulus, keeping the decision component constant. It was found that in both conditions RT2 was delayed, suggesting that the decision component was a part of the single channel. However, RT2 was delayed by a significantly greater amount if a motor response was required, indicating that the motor component is part of the single channel as well. Implications of the results for an expectancy theory of the psychological refractory period are discussed. 相似文献
290.
Three different coding schemes, i.e., transformations between stimuli and responses, were used in a continuous information transmission situation. The stimuli were groups of binary digits and S was required to transform these into verbal equivalents of combinations formed by taking various sets of the English letters. Three codes, one based on communication theory, one based on Miller’s (1956) chunking hypothesis, and a combination of the first two were used. In addition, two levels of stimulus redundancy, zero and 0.5 were used. Time to complete the task and errors were the de-pendent variables. The Ss were run for twelve days. The basic result was that Ss transmitted information at a constant rate regardless of the code or redundancy level used. 相似文献