全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3507篇 |
免费 | 37篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
3545篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 21篇 |
2022年 | 45篇 |
2021年 | 48篇 |
2020年 | 80篇 |
2019年 | 97篇 |
2018年 | 137篇 |
2017年 | 134篇 |
2016年 | 171篇 |
2015年 | 82篇 |
2014年 | 93篇 |
2013年 | 426篇 |
2012年 | 126篇 |
2011年 | 132篇 |
2010年 | 101篇 |
2009年 | 130篇 |
2008年 | 122篇 |
2007年 | 139篇 |
2006年 | 111篇 |
2005年 | 104篇 |
2004年 | 85篇 |
2003年 | 81篇 |
2002年 | 72篇 |
2001年 | 55篇 |
2000年 | 49篇 |
1999年 | 43篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 46篇 |
1996年 | 58篇 |
1995年 | 64篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 37篇 |
1992年 | 29篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 28篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1983年 | 28篇 |
1982年 | 45篇 |
1981年 | 29篇 |
1980年 | 33篇 |
1979年 | 25篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 18篇 |
1975年 | 22篇 |
1974年 | 25篇 |
1973年 | 45篇 |
排序方式: 共有3545条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Emeritus Professor GRAHAM R. DAVIDSON KATE E. MURRAY ROBERT SCHWEITZER 《Australian psychologist》2008,43(3):160-174
This review covers refugee mental health and wellbeing within the Australian context to assist psychologists who provide services for, or who conduct research with, refugees. It provides a brief overview of the refugee movement, prior to examining evidence relating to the impacts of pre‐displacement, displacement and resettlement factors on psychological adjustment in the resettlement phase, and the systemic and sociopolitical factors that influence the process of adaptation during resettlement. Australian findings suggest that mental health and wellbeing outcomes are influenced by a complexity of pre‐displacement, displacement, resettlement and systemic factors; the detention or award of temporary residence to refugees who are already experiencing psychological distress on arrival are cases in point. Limitations of the findings are considered. More research into the combined, pathwise relations between the psychosocial pre‐determinants and psychological sequelae of the refugee experience is required. Finally, suggestions for the development of practice, training and ethical guidelines are offered. 相似文献
992.
Professor Kathy Sylva Stephen Scott Vasiliki Totsika Katharina Ereky‐Stevens Carolyn Crook 《The British journal of educational psychology》2008,78(3):435-455
Background . Low levels of literacy and high levels of behaviour problems in middle childhood often co‐occur. These persistent difficulties pose a risk to academic and social development, leading to social exclusion in adulthood. Although parent‐training programmes have been shown to be effective in enabling parents to support their children's development, very few parent interventions offer a combination of behavioural and literacy training. Aims . This paper (1) reports on a prevention programme which aimed to tackle behaviour and literacy problems in children at the beginning of school, and (2) presents the effects of the intervention on children's literacy. Sample . One hundred and four 5‐ and 6‐year‐old children selected from eight schools in an inner city disadvantaged community in London participated in the intervention. Methods . This is a randomized control trial with pre‐ and post‐measurements designed to evaluate the effectiveness of an intervention. The behavioural intervention consisted of the ‘Incredible Years’ group parenting programme combined with a new programme designed to train parents to support their children's reading at home. Results . Analyses demonstrated a significant effect of the intervention on children's word reading and writing skills, as well as parents' use of reading strategies with their children. Conclusion . A structured multicomponent preventive package delivered with attention to fidelity can enable parents to support their children's reading at home and increase their literacy skills. Together with the improvement in child behaviour, these changes could improve the life chances of children in disadvantaged communities. 相似文献
993.
Generating solutions to anagrams leads to a memory advantage for those solutions, with generated words remembered better than words simply read. However, an additional advantage is not typically found for solutions to difficult anagrams relative to solutions to easy ones, presenting a challenge for the cognitive effort explanation of the generation effect. In the present series of experiments, the effect of manipulating anagram difficulty is explored further by introducing two new source-monitoring judgments. These studies demonstrate that when attention is directed at test to the operations activated during encoding (by way of source-monitoring judgments focused on solving vs. constructing anagrams), a source advantage is observed for difficult anagrams. However, when attention is directed to the anagrams themselves, asking participants to remember the kinds of anagrams generated or solved (based on kind of rule rather than subjective impressions of difficulty), a similar source advantage is not observed. The present studies bring a new perspective to the investigation of difficulty manipulations on memory for problem solving by illustrating the impact of a shift in focus from the effort mediating cognitive operations to specifics about the cognitive operations themselves. 相似文献
994.
Susan A. Basow Kristen F. Cahill Julie E. Phelan Kathryn Longshore Ann McGillicuddy-DeLisi 《Psychology of women quarterly》2007,31(1):85-95
This study examined gendered perceptions of relational and physical aggressive behaviors and personal experiences with both types of aggression. Prior research suggested that physical aggression by males and relational aggression by females would be perceived most negatively. College students (186 female, 128 male) rated the acceptability and harmfulness of aggression in scenarios in which type of aggression and perpetrator and target gender were varied. As predicted, relational aggression by female characters and physical aggression by male characters were rated as less acceptable and more aggressive/harmful than the same behavior by the other gender. Thus, expectations regarding the gender-appropriateness of aggression appear to affect perceptions of such acts. Despite popular conceptions that females use and are harmed by relational aggression more than males, there was no gender difference in experience with relational aggression, nor were female targets viewed as more harmed by such aggression than their male counterparts. 相似文献
995.
Simultaneous prompting, a type of antecedent prompt and test procedure, was used to teach the chained task of making juice from a frozen concentrate using a pictorial recipe to four elementary-aged students with moderate intellectual disabilities. Using a multiple probe design, all students acquired the task and maintained the behavior with at least 85% accuracy up to 60 days after training was terminated. In addition, students generalized the skill across settings and materials. The students' ability to expressively identify the words shown on the recipe cards was tested prior to and after training. Although not directly trained, three of the four students increased their ability to read the words. 相似文献
996.
Professor Robert M. Baird Ph.D. 《Journal of religion and health》1994,33(4):305-312
I reflect on certain random events that befall us and to those we love, random events we never would have chosen because of
the pain and suffering they involve. A distinction is drawn between the bad luck of physical assaults, such as genetic defects
and the bad luck growing out of value conflicts. Illuminated by the writings of two philosophers— Wilcox and Wolterstorff—I
examine the story of Job's maturing response to bad luck. I conclude with some reflections on Christ's response to pain and
suffering and its implications for our response to bad luck. 相似文献
997.
Alice J. O’toole Kenneth A. Deffenbacher Dominique Valentin Herve Abdi 《Memory & cognition》1994,22(2):208-224
The other-race effect was examined in a series of experiments and simulations that looked at the relationships among observer ratings of typicality, familiarity, attractiveness, memorability, and the performance variables ofd’ and criterion. Experiment 1 replicated the other-race effect with our Caucasian and Japanese stimuli for both Caucasian and Asian observers. In Experiment 2, we collected ratings from Caucasian observers on the faces used in the recognition task. A Varimax-rotated principal components analysis on the rating and performance data for the Caucasian faces replicated Vokey and Read’s (1992) finding that typicality is composed of two orthogonal components, dissociable via their independent relationships to: (1) attractiveness and familiarity ratings and (2) memorahility ratings. For Japanese faces, however, we fond that typicality was related only to memorahility. Where performance measures were concerned, two additional principal components dominated by criterion and byd’ emerged for Caucasian faces. For the Japanese faces, however, the performance measures ofd’ and criterion merged into a single component that represented a second component of typicality, one orthogonal to thememorability-dominated component. A measure offace representation quality extracted from an autoassociative neural network trained with a majority of Caucasian faces and a minority of Japanese faces was incorporated into the principal components analysis. For both Caucasian and Japanese faces, the neural network measure related both to memorability ratings and to human accuracy measures. Combined, the human data and simulation results indicate that the memorahility component of typicality may be related to small, local, distinctive features, whereas the attractiveness/familiarity component may be more related to the global, shape-based properties of the face. 相似文献
998.
999.
1000.
This study investigated talker-dependent and talker-independent perceptual adaptation to foreign-accent English. Experiment 1 investigated talker-dependent adaptation by comparing native English listeners' recognition accuracy for Chinese-accented English across single and multiple talker presentation conditions. Results showed that the native listeners adapted to the foreign-accented speech over the course of the single talker presentation condition with some variation in the rate and extent of this adaptation depending on the baseline sentence intelligibility of the foreign-accented talker. Experiment 2 investigated talker-independent perceptual adaptation to Chinese-accented English by exposing native English listeners to Chinese-accented English and then testing their perception of English produced by a novel Chinese-accented talker. Results showed that, if exposed to multiple talkers of Chinese-accented English during training, native English listeners could achieve talker-independent adaptation to Chinese-accented English. Taken together, these findings provide evidence for highly flexible speech perception processes that can adapt to speech that deviates substantially from the pronunciation norms in the native talker community along multiple acoustic-phonetic dimensions. 相似文献