首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   180916篇
  免费   8087篇
  国内免费   157篇
  2021年   1565篇
  2020年   2890篇
  2019年   3533篇
  2018年   3710篇
  2017年   4148篇
  2016年   4803篇
  2015年   4020篇
  2014年   4927篇
  2013年   23950篇
  2012年   4893篇
  2011年   4026篇
  2010年   4040篇
  2009年   4892篇
  2008年   4104篇
  2007年   3653篇
  2006年   4203篇
  2005年   4126篇
  2004年   3641篇
  2003年   3245篇
  2002年   3097篇
  2001年   3683篇
  2000年   3592篇
  1999年   3365篇
  1998年   2885篇
  1997年   2706篇
  1996年   2608篇
  1995年   2447篇
  1994年   2423篇
  1993年   2365篇
  1992年   2770篇
  1991年   2574篇
  1990年   2469篇
  1989年   2390篇
  1988年   2350篇
  1987年   2352篇
  1986年   2349篇
  1985年   2535篇
  1984年   2612篇
  1983年   2377篇
  1982年   2412篇
  1981年   2368篇
  1980年   2195篇
  1979年   2298篇
  1978年   2241篇
  1977年   2193篇
  1976年   1968篇
  1975年   2096篇
  1974年   2208篇
  1973年   2003篇
  1972年   1644篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
242.
243.
244.
Feminist theory holds that many of the pathological behaviors observed in patients result from their position in the social hierarchy. The goals of the demonstration detailed in this article are to show the impact of current gender roles on the psychological well-being of women and men and to generate understanding and discussion of the problems that relative status can create in the therapeutic relationship. This teaching demonstration draws parallels between the Zimbardo (1971) prison experiment and the impact that assignment to low-and high-power roles can have on the psychological health of women and men. Students are asked to rate men and women on some of the diagnostic criteria that the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) associates with Axis I and II diagnoses. The students watch a video of Zimbardo's prison study and then rate the prisoners and guards. This demonstration provides another explanation for gender patterns in psychopathology by demonstrating that psychologically healthy White males will develop different patterns of psychopathology depending on whether they are placed in a dominant or subordinate role. When placed in the subordinate role, they exhibit behaviors typically seen in women clients; when placed in a dominant role they exhibit behaviors commonly associated with male clients. This exercise demonstrates the unhealthy nature of the gender roles for both women and men.  相似文献   
245.
Using both cross-sectional and longitudinal data spanning a decade, this article analyzes how dietary behavior of household children and adults (N = 576 households) was affected by the Stanford Five-City Project (FCP). Tests of a three-part, cumulative model of bidirectional influences within the family, which conceptualizes household members as sources of influence on each other and subject to influence by an external agent, were supported. Children and adults were influenced by both each other and the FCP campaign in changing and maintaining health behaviors. This article demonstrates that public health campaigns can be made more effective if they conceptualize both children and adults as potential sources of influence. The long-term effectiveness of such efforts can be enhanced by encouraging families, as opposed to individuals, to change health behaviors.  相似文献   
246.
247.
A review is presented of empirical research on the effects of gender and power on the use of influence strategies in interpersonal relationships. Several variables are considered, including gender, power, status, relationship of agent to target, and the goal of the influence attempt. Although gender appears to account for some of the findings, power and status are more critical variables in choice of power strategies. Because gender is inextricably linked to power and status, the relationship of gender to influence strategy usage can only be understood in terms of its relationship to power and status.  相似文献   
248.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
249.
250.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号