全文获取类型
收费全文 | 421篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
429篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 9篇 |
2016年 | 7篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 35篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 7篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有429条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
92.
93.
Hostile attributional biases in severely aggressive adolescents 总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8
Adolescent boys (N = 128) from a maximum security prison for juvenile offenders were administered a task to assess hostile attributional biases. As hypothesized, these biases were positively correlated with undersocialized aggressive conduct disorder (as indicated by high scores on standardized scales and by psychiatric diagnoses), with reactive-aggressive behavior, and with the number of interpersonally violent crimes committed. Hostile attributional biases were found not to relate to nonviolent crimes or to socialized aggressive behavior disorder. These findings held even when race and estimates of intelligence and socioeconomic status were controlled. These findings suggest that within a population of juvenile offenders, attributional biases are implicated specifically in interpersonal reactive aggression that involves anger and not in socialized delinquency. 相似文献
94.
The development of math skills is a critical component of early education and a strong indicator of later school and economic success. Recent research utilizing population‐normed, standardized measures of math achievement suggest that structural and functional integrity of parietal regions, especially the intraparietal sulcus, are closely related to the development of math skills. However, it is unknown how these findings relate to in‐school math learning. The present study is the first to address this issue by investigating the relationship between regional differences in grey matter (GM) volume and performance in grade‐level mathematics as measured by a state‐wide, school‐based test of math achievement (TCAP math) in children from 3rd to 8th grade. Results show that increased GM volume in the bilateral hippocampal formation and the right inferior frontal gyrus, regions associated with learning and memory, is associated with higher TCAP math scores. Secondary analyses revealed that GM volume in the left angular gyrus had a stronger relationship to TCAP math in grades 3–4 than in grades 5–8 while the relationship between GM volume in the left inferior frontal gyrus and TCAP math was stronger for grades 5–8. These results suggest that the neuroanatomical architecture related to in‐school math achievement differs from that related to math achievement measured by standardized tests, and that the most related neural structures differ as a function of grade level. We suggest, therefore, that the use of school‐relevant outcome measures is critical if neuroscience is to bridge the gap to education. 相似文献
95.
Nicola S. Schutte John M. Malouff Ian Price Samantha Walter Greg Burke Catherine Wilkinson 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2008,27(2):102-111
Two studies applied a person–situation model to examine the effect of emotional affordances of situations. Participants rated
their emotional functioning as more extensive in situations classified as being high in emotional affordance than those classified
as low in emotional affordance. Participants who scored higher on the individual difference characteristic of emotional intelligence
were more interested in entering high emotional affordance situations than were individuals lower in emotional intelligence,
and participants who scored higher on emotional intelligence were rated by others as being more successful in high emotional
affordance situations than individuals lower in emotional intelligence. These results provide preliminary evidence that the
interaction between emotional intelligence and situations may influence emotional functioning. 相似文献
96.
97.
98.
99.
Bevan Adrian Craig Darren Peter Morton Lillian Marton Kent Alva Barry Gane Terry Leslie Butler Paul Meredith Rankin Kevin Ross Price 《Journal of religion and health》2018,57(3):994-1009
Students attending Seventh-day Adventist (Adventist) schools in Australia have been shown to have better health status and behaviours compared to secular norms, yet these schools cater for a high percentage of non-Adventist students. The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of religious affiliation (Adventist/non-Adventist) on the health status and behaviours of students attending Adventist secondary schools in Australia. The sample included 1734 students who responded to a health and lifestyle survey that captured demographic details, self-reported height and weight, self-reported health status, mental health and select health behaviours. Students who identified themselves as Adventist reported significantly better health behaviours than the non-Adventist students in several behavioural domains, especially among the male students. However, this did not translate to a difference in health status. Further research is needed to understand the causal mechanisms responsible for the potential health advantage of Adventist students, which may include family or church religious influences. 相似文献
100.
Suggestibility effects persist after one year in children who experienced a single or repeated event
Heather L. Price Deborah A. Connolly 《Journal of applied research in memory and cognition》2013,2(2):89-94
The study of long-term memory for repeated events has important implications for understanding autobiographical memory in a forensic context. Recall accuracy and suggestibility for details of an instance of a repeated event versus a single event were examined in children aged 5–6 and 7–8 years after a one-year delay. Children who reported an instance of a repeated event were more likely to report that a non-experienced detail had occurred and reported less correct information than did single-event children. After one year a significant suggestibility effect was still present. The present experiment provides further evidence for both the capabilities and limitations of children's long-term recall and reinforces the importance of non-suggestive interviews of children at all stages of investigation. 相似文献