全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3902篇 |
免费 | 67篇 |
专业分类
3969篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 24篇 |
2023年 | 43篇 |
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 47篇 |
2020年 | 96篇 |
2019年 | 131篇 |
2018年 | 140篇 |
2017年 | 161篇 |
2016年 | 155篇 |
2015年 | 110篇 |
2014年 | 141篇 |
2013年 | 487篇 |
2012年 | 188篇 |
2011年 | 219篇 |
2010年 | 123篇 |
2009年 | 144篇 |
2008年 | 192篇 |
2007年 | 134篇 |
2006年 | 147篇 |
2005年 | 130篇 |
2004年 | 142篇 |
2003年 | 125篇 |
2002年 | 109篇 |
2001年 | 44篇 |
2000年 | 47篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 23篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 27篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 21篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 25篇 |
1986年 | 27篇 |
1985年 | 24篇 |
1984年 | 31篇 |
1983年 | 33篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 27篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 21篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有3969条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
Kenny Coventry Simon Venn Graham Smith Andrew Morley 《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2013,25(1):71-100
Three experiments investigated the role of object knowledge on participants' ability to solve a spatial arrangement problem. The task was to rearrange six real-world three-dimensional objects so that their relative locations agreed with a given set of rules. The aim of the experiments was to tease out the relative extent to which object association, orientation, and object-specific functional relations affect performance on arrangement tasks. When the problem was presented vertically (objects arranged in piles), participants solved functional canonical versions of the problem significantly quicker than functional non-canonical versions both between (Experiments 1a and 2), and within subjects (Experiment 3). When the arrangement problem was presented horizontally (objects arranged flat in two rows), no significant differences in solution times were found between conditions (Experiments 1b and 2). Overall the results provide evidence for the importance of object-specific functional relations as a predictor of the solution time of spatial arrangement problems, although some differences were noted between single and multiple presentation of problems when specific rules within problems were rotated. The importance of functional information in memory as a constraint on the building of mental models and problem spaces is discussed. 相似文献
952.
Catriona M. Morrison Katherine W. Hirsh Tameron Chappell Andrew W. Ellis 《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2013,25(4):435-459
An important determinant of picture and word naming speed is the age at which the names were learned (age of acquisition). Two related interpretations of these effects are that they reflect differences between words in their cumulative frequency of use, or that they reflect differences in the amount of time early and lateacquired words have spent in lexical memory. Both theories predict that differences between early and late-acquired words will be less apparent in older than younger adults. Two experiments are reported in which younger and older adults read words varying in age of acquisition or frequency, or named objects varying in age of acquisition. There was an observed effect of word frequency only for young adults' word naming. In contrast, strong age of acquisition effects were found for both the young and the old participants. The implications of these results for theories of how age of acquisition might affect lexical processing are discussed. 相似文献
953.
Continental Philosophy Review - In marked contrast to Husserlian “unities of sense” that structure consciousness around egoic ideal-meaning intention, contemporary phenomenology orders... 相似文献
954.
955.
Applied Research in Quality of Life - 相似文献
956.
Nathan A. Gillespie C. Robert Cloninger Andrew C. Heath Nicholas G. Martin 《Personality and individual differences》2003,35(8):1931-1946
The purpose of this study was to determine whether Cloninger's revised 7-factor model of personality showed incremental validity over his four dimensions of temperament. A sample of 2517 Australian twins aged over 50 between 1993 and 1995 returned completed self-reported measures of Self-directedness, Cooperativeness, and Self-transcendence from Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory. Many of these twins had participated in a 1988 study containing Cloninger's temperament measures of Harm Avoidance, Novelty Seeking, Reward Dependence and Persistence. Contrary to theoretical expectations, univariate analyses revealed that familial aggregation for the character dimensions could be entirely explained by additive gene action alone. Although temperament explained 26, 37 and 10% of additive genetic variance in Self-directedness, Cooperativeness and Self-transcendence, respectively, seven genetic factors were required to explain the genetic variance among the TPQ dimensions, and almost all of the non-shared environmental variance was unique to each dimension of character. Our results indicate that the inclusion of all seven dimensions in a taxonomy of personality is warranted. 相似文献
957.
Andrew Cullison 《Philosophical Studies》2007,136(3):305-318
By exploiting a concept called ways of believing, I offer a plausible reformulation of the doctrine of privileged access.
This reformulation will provide us with a defense of compatibilism, the view that content externalism and privileged access
are compatible. 相似文献
958.
Although cross-sectional studies have shown a reliable association between marital status and subjective well-being, a recent longitudinal study [Lucas et al. 2003, Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 84(3), pp.␣527–539] found no support for the idea that happiness increases after marriage. Instead, participants who got married reported short-term increases followed by complete adaptation back to baseline levels of well-being. However, researchers have criticized this study on two grounds. First, these results contradict cohort-based analyses from a nationally representative sample. Second, these analyses do not control for pre-marriage cohabitation, which could potentially inflate baseline levels of well-being. The original data (plus four additional waves) are reanalyzed to address these concerns. Results confirm that individuals do not get a lasting boost in life satisfaction following marriage.The data used in this paper were made available by the German Institute for Economic Research (DIW). This research was supported by a grant from the Anthony Marchionne Foundation. 相似文献
959.
Dora Matzke Matthew Hughes Johanna C. Badcock Patricia Michie Andrew Heathcote 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2017,79(4):1078-1086
We used Bayesian cognitive modelling to identify the underlying causes of apparent inhibitory deficits in the stop-signal paradigm. The analysis was applied to stop-signal data reported by Badcock et al. (Psychological Medicine 32: 87-297, 2002) and Hughes et al. (Biological Psychology 89: 220-231, 2012), where schizophrenia patients and control participants made rapid choice responses, but on some trials were signalled to stop their ongoing response. Previous research has assumed an inhibitory deficit in schizophrenia, because estimates of the mean time taken to react to the stop signal are longer in patients than controls. We showed that these longer estimates are partly due to failing to react to the stop signal (“trigger failures”) and partly due to a slower initiation of inhibition, implicating a failure of attention rather than a deficit in the inhibitory process itself. Correlations between the probability of trigger failures and event-related potentials reported by Hughes et al. are interpreted as supporting the attentional account of inhibitory deficits. Our results, and those of Matzke et al. (2016), who report that controls also display a substantial although lower trigger-failure rate, indicate that attentional factors need to be taken into account when interpreting results from the stop-signal paradigm. 相似文献
960.