全文获取类型
收费全文 | 159篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
166篇 |
出版年
2018年 | 3篇 |
2017年 | 3篇 |
2016年 | 2篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 2篇 |
2013年 | 3篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 4篇 |
2007年 | 5篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 3篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
1959年 | 5篇 |
1958年 | 3篇 |
1957年 | 4篇 |
1956年 | 3篇 |
1955年 | 3篇 |
1954年 | 4篇 |
1953年 | 3篇 |
1952年 | 3篇 |
1951年 | 2篇 |
1950年 | 1篇 |
1949年 | 1篇 |
1948年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有166条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Howard R. Pollio Peter R. Jensen Michael A. O’Neil 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2014,43(1):81-104
Three empirical studies and one analysis of pre-existing data were performed to determine the everyday meanings of time, a major component of phenomenological analyses of human experience. To this end, participants in Studies 1 and 2 sorted time related words into groups having similar meanings, with these groups then evaluated by hierarchical clustering procedures. Results of Studies 1 and 2 produced similar clustering patterns suggesting it was possible to define the everyday meanings of time in terms of experiences of change and continuity, linear organization, tempo, and boundaries. Results of Study 3 indicated little or no effect on clustering patterns of time words when space words also were included in the set of items to be sorted. Concerns about the size and representativeness of the words used as stimuli in Studies 1, 2 and 3 led to an analysis of over 2,000 words falling under the general heading of Time in Roget’s Thesaurus. Results of this analysis revealed that clusters comparable to those obtained in the other three studies also appeared in these data. These results were discussed in terms of their implications for the way in which “invisible” concepts such as time are thought about and used, particularly as related to figurative expression. 相似文献
62.
63.
STEVE DE SHAZER 《Family process》1984,23(1):11-17
For any conceptual distinction to be useful ( 5 ) within the field of family therapy, it needs to lead to some clear answers to the question: What does this distinction mean for clinical practice? The distinction between (a) the family-as-a-system, and (b) family-therapy-as-a-system leads to a clinical perspective, or stance, that includes a focus on changing. Once this focus is clear, the therapist can help to create the expectation of changing and consequently promote changing. That is, techniques can be developed using positive feedback loops. 1 1 It is important to remember throughout this essay that “positive” and “negative feedback loops,”“deviation-amplifying,”“deviation-counteracting processes,” etc., are just heuristic devices or metaphors. Human systems do not have such things. Human systems are described by an observer “as if.”
Moreover, this distinction leads to a therapeutic stance in which not changing is a surprise. 相似文献
Moreover, this distinction leads to a therapeutic stance in which not changing is a surprise. 相似文献
64.
Marian K. Pitts Michael K. Smith Howard R. Pollio 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1982,11(4):347-368
Three current theories of metaphor production, those of atribute matching, analogical reasoning, and Gestalt perceptual processing, were tested by asking subjects to compare famous people with various categories. Three sets of instructions were devised, each reflecting one of the major theories. Results showed that while subjects could make comparisons under all conditions, they found it significantly easier to work with Gestalt instructions than with either of the other two. Gestalt instructions also produced more novel metaphoric comparisons, with such responses judged to be no less apt than statistically more conventional comparisons. Similar results did not apply to metaphoric comparisons produced under the remaining two instructional conditions where statistically novel comparisons were judged to be less apt than their more conventional counterparts. A second study was run to control for possible demand property differences among the various instructional conditions. Even with these additional controls, results of the second study replicated all major findings reported in Experiment I. Implications of these findings for the production of metaphors in more natural contexts were discussed, as were implications for creative problem-solving. In both cases it was suggested that a direct, nonananalytic approach might be more useful in revealing unexpected and important relationships between concepts than would be the case for more analytic approaches usually stressed as optimum for either metaphor production or problem solving.Visiting Research Scholar (1979–1980) 相似文献
65.
66.
RIASEC TYPES AND BIG FIVE TRAITS AS PREDICTORS OF EMPLOYMENT STATUS AND NATURE OF EMPLOYMENT 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
This prospective study investigated the validity of the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality and Holland's RIASEC vocational interest typology in predicting employment status and the nature of employment in a sample of graduating college seniors as they entered the job market. A sample of 934 senior college graduates enrolled in various academic subjects filled in Costa and McCrae's NEO-PI-R (1992) and Holland's Self-Directed Search (1979). One year after graduation, they were requested to describe their labor market positions and jobs, using the Position Classification Inventory (PCI; Gottfredson & Holland, 1991). Six hundred and twelve people responded to the second call, of whom 335 were employed and 66 unemployed. The incremental validity of the 2 models over and above each other was investigated in the sample of employed and unemployed subjects ( N = 401) using stepwise regression analysis. The results showed that Extraversion and Conscientiousness were the only valid predictors of employment status and that vocational interests did not show incremental validity over and above these factors. The RIASEC types, however, were clearly superior in explaining the nature of employment, underscoring the validity of Holland's hexagonal calculus assumptions. Employment reflecting Realistic, Social and Enterprising characteristics was to a limited extent predicted by four of the Big Five, except Neuroticism, over and above the RIASEC types. The findings are discussed in the framework of Schneider's Attraction-Selection-Attrition (ASA) theory (1987) concluding that Holland's RIASEC model is more employee-driven, being better at predicting the nature of employment, whereas the FFM is more employer-oriedted, with greater validity in evaluating the employability and employment status of applicants. 相似文献
67.
In order to determine whether mechanically generated sentences, often referred to in the literature as anomalous, are in fact anomalous, subjects in the present experiment were asked to do two different tasks: a paired-associates learning task involving anomalous sentences as responses, and a second task in which they were asked to interpret such sentences. These tasks were counterbalanced across different groups of subjects. Results of the interpretation task showed that a large proportion of subjects were able to give interpretations for anomalous sentences, while learning results indicated that anomalous sentences were more difficult to anticipate than natural sentencesonly when task order was Learn first, Interpret second. In the order Interpret-Learn, differences in ease of learning between anomalous and naturally occurring sentences did not appear. The results of this study demonstrate that anomalous sentences are interpretable, that a range of difficulty for such sentences can be established, that anomalous sentences are learned as easily as naturally occurring ones after interpretation, and that many of the interpretations given to such sentences are metaphoric in character. These findings are discussed in terms of their implications for models of lexical organization. 相似文献
68.
69.
70.
This research examined the influence of negative political advertising frames on the thoughts and feelings people generate in response to campaign advertising. Preparing and conducting this investigation involved the use of a multiple‐method strategy. Content analysis identified two advertising frames (i.e., candidate theme and ad hoc issue advertisements) and two experiments separately induced political cynicism and politician accountability. Three hundred and sixty people participated in the experimental studies, in which they read and responded, using a thought‐listing technique, to candidate theme or ad hoc issue negative advertisements. Results demonstrated that participants were more likely to generate cynical comments and hold politicians accountable for the country's ills when reading candidate theme advertisements than ad hoc issue advertisements. The results indicate that this contributes to a political climate of cynicism and may function to erode the electorate's overall trust in government. 相似文献