全文获取类型
收费全文 | 608篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
618篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 65篇 |
2012年 | 20篇 |
2011年 | 27篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 26篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 23篇 |
2005年 | 32篇 |
2004年 | 18篇 |
2003年 | 32篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有618条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
461.
Pierre Le Morvan 《Erkenntnis》2002,56(2):151-168
Crispin Sartwell ingeniously defends the provocative thesis that mere true belief suffices for knowledge. In doing so, he challenges one of the most deeply entrenched epistemological tenets, namely that knowledge must be more than mere true belief. Particularly interesting is the way he defends his thesis by appealing to considerations adduced by such prominent epistemologists as William Alston, Laurence BonJour, Alvin Goldman and Paul Moser, each of whom denies that knowledge is merely true belief. In this paper, I argue that the case Sartwell presents for his thesis fails. However, by examining why it fails, we may derive at least four important epistemological lessons: (1) being justified does not entail being able to give a justification; (2) we should distinguish between epistemic justification conceived of as intrinsically conducive to truth and conceived of as extrinsically conducive to truth; (3) we should distinguish between epistemic justification conceived of as an essential criterion of knowledge and conceived of as an accidental criterion of knowledge; and (4) epistemologists need to specify how the telos of inquiry involves more than the acquisition of (merely) true beliefs.Socrates: Then tell me: what definition can we give with the least risk of contradicting ourselves?Theaetetus: The one we tried before, Socrates. I have nothing else to suggest.Socrates: What was that?Theaetetus: That true belief is knowledge. Surely there can at least be no mistake in believing what is true and the consequences are always satisfactory.Theaetetus 200d–e 相似文献
462.
Gaillard V Barrouillet P Jarrold C Camos V 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2011,110(3):469-479
Several models assume that working memory development depends on age-related increases in efficiency and speed of processing. However, age-related increases in the efficiency of the mechanisms that counteract forgetting and restore memory traces may also be important. This hypothesis was tested in three experiments by manipulating both the processing duration within a working memory task and the time available to restore memory traces. Third- and sixth-grade children performed a complex span task in which they maintained series of letters while adding numbers to series of digits. When we equated processing and restoration times between ages, the developmental difference in working memory span was reduced but remained significant. However, this residual difference was eliminated when the time available to reactivate memory traces was tailored to the processing speed of each age group. This indicates that children employ active mechanisms for maintenance and restoration of memory traces that develop with age. 相似文献
463.
The present study investigates the emotional determinants of the phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories. A total of 84 participants completed the Memory Characteristics Questionnaire (MCQ, Johnson, Foley, Suengas, & Raye, 1988) after retrieving and orally describing a negative, a positive, and a neutral autobiographical memory. In addition, self-report and physiological measures of emotional state at retrieval were recorded. Results suggest that recall of perceptual, sensory, and semantic elements is better for emotional memories than for neutral ones. This difference is not significant for contextual and temporal aspects, suggesting that emotional memories are more vivid but no more specific than are neutral ones. In addition, positive memories yielded higher MCQ ratings than did negative memories for sensory, temporal, and contextual aspects. Finally, correlations suggest a positive relation between emotional state at retrieval and level of phenomenal detail of retrieved memories. Results are interpreted in terms of multilevel models of emotion and of Conway and Pleydell-Pearce's (2000) model. 相似文献
464.
Pierre Le Morvan 《Erkenntnis》2005,62(2):145-155
Alvin Goldman contends that, in addition to the familiar sense or use of the term “knowledge” according to which knowledge is at least true justified belief, there is a weaker yet strict sense or use of the term “knowledge” according to which knowledge amounts to nothing more than information-possession or mere true belief. In this paper, I argue that Goldman has failed to show that there is such a weaker sense, and that, even if he had shown this, he has not shown that this putative weaker sense is a strict one by his own criterion for strictness. 相似文献
465.
Pierre M. Balthazar 《Pastoral Psychology》2007,55(5):537-542
Following the work of Stevens Rogers (2002), this article starts with the premise that humanity can be seen as a child growing in changing relationship with his or
her mother. With Jesus representing humanity, the relationship between a mother and a child can be an archetype for understanding
the changing relationship between the Virgin Mary and humanity as narrated in the Gospels and the Book of Revelation. With
the Annunciation by the Angel, the Boy Jesus in the Temple, the wedding at Cana, and Mary at the foot of the cross as developmental
stages, this article shows the changing relationship between humanity and the Virgin Mary.
Pierre M. Balthazar is a student in the PsyD program in Clinical Psychology at the Illinois School of Professional Psychology. 相似文献
466.
Phonological developmental dyslexics remain impaired in phonetic categorical perception (CP) even in adulthood. We studied the brain correlates of CP in dyslexics and controls using a block design fMRI protocol and stimuli from an phonetic continuum between natural /Pa/ and /Ta/ syllables. Subjects performed a pseudo-passive listening task which does not imply voluntary categorical judgment. In the control group, categorical deviant stimuli elicited specific activations in the left angular gyrus, the right inferior frontal gyrus and the right superior cingulate cortex. These regions were not activated in the dyslexic group in which activation was observed for acoustic but not phonetic changes in stimuli. Failures to activate key regions for language perception and auditory attention in dyslexic might account for persistent deficits in phonological awareness and reading tasks. 相似文献
467.
Foreground contextual fear memory consolidation requires two independent phases of hippocampal ERK/CREB activation
下载免费PDF全文

Trifilieff P Herry C Vanhoutte P Caboche J Desmedt A Riedel G Mons N Micheau J 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2006,13(3):349-358
Fear conditioning is a popular model for investigating physiological and cellular mechanisms of memory formation. In this paradigm, a footshock is either systematically associated to a tone (paired conditioning) or is pseudorandomly distributed (unpaired conditioning). In the former procedure, the tone/shock association is acquired, whereas in the latter procedure, the context/shock association will prevail. Animals with chronically implanted recording electrodes show enhanced amplitude of the extracellularly recorded field EPSP in CA1 pyramidal cells for up to 24 h after unpaired, but not paired, fear conditioning. This is paralleled by a differential activation of the ERK/CREB pathway in CA1, which is monophasic in paired conditioning (0-15 min post-conditioning), but biphasic (0-1 h and 9-12 h post-conditioning) in unpaired conditioning as revealed by immunocytochemistry and Western blotting. Intrahippocampal injection of the MEK inhibitor U0126 prior to each phase prevents the activation of both ERK1/2 and CREB after unpaired conditioning. Block of any activation phase leads to memory impairment. We finally reveal that the biphasic activation of ERK/CREB activity is independently regulated, yet both phases are critically required for the consolidation of long-term memories following unpaired fear conditioning. These data provide compelling evidence that CA1 serves different forms of memory by expressing differential cellular mechanisms that are dependent on the training regime. 相似文献
468.
Super's theory of vocational development, initially formulated in the 1950s, has led to a number of important research programmes, focusing particularly on the concept of vocational maturity. The major findings of these studies are surveyed in this paper. They suggest that vocational maturity is a development characteristic which increases with age, is multidimensional in nature, develops at different rates in different individuals, and can help in the pre-diction of occupational satisfaction, occupational success, and career success. 相似文献
469.
Pierre R. Dasen 《International journal of psychology》1975,10(3):165-180
Le développement des opérations concrètes est étudié dans l'optique piagé-tienne chez des enfants esquimaux canadiens âgés de 6 à 14 ans. Les résultats sont comparés à ceux d'une étude similaire chez des enfants aborigènes australiens et européens. Les stades et leur ordre hiérarchique se retrouvent de façon identique dans toutes les populations comparées, alors que le rythme de développement est affecté par des variables socio-culturelles. Chez les enfants esquimaux, les courbes de développement dans le domaine des conservations apparaissent asymptotiques, une plus ou moins large partie de l'échantillon n'atteignant pas le stade des opérations concrètes, alors que, dans le domaine des relations et des classes, et surtout dans le domaine spatial, il y a simple décalage de rythme par rapport aux enfants européens, ou développement chronologiquement semblable. Une partie des résultats est discutée en fonction de la distinction entre compétence et performance. 相似文献
470.
Wolff M Benhassine N Costet P Hen R Segu L Buhot MC 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2003,10(5):401-409
Serotonin (5-HT) plays a modulatory role in mnemonic functions, especially by interacting with the cholinergic system. The 5-HT1B receptor is a key target of this interaction. The 5-HT1B receptor knockout mice were found previously to exhibit a facilitation in hippocampal-dependent spatial reference memory learning. In the present study, we submitted mice to a delayed spatial working memory task, allowing the introduction of various delays between an exposure trial and a test trial. The 5-HT1BKO and wild-type mice learned the task in a radial-arm water maze (returning to the most recent presented arm containing the escape platform), and exhibited a high level of performance at delays of 0 and 5 min. However, at the delay of 60 min, only 5-HT1BKO mice exhibited an impairment. At a delay of 90 min, all mice were impaired. Treatment by scopolamine (0.8 mg/kg) induced the same pattern of performance in wild type as did the mutation for short (5 min, no impairment) and long (60 min, impairment) delays. The 22-month-old wild-type and knockout mice exhibited an impairment at short delays (5 and 15 min). The effect of the mutation affected both young-adult and aged mice at delays of 15, 30, and 60 min. Neurobiological data show that stimulation of the 5-HT1B receptor inhibits the release of acetylcholine in the hippocampus, but stimulates this in the frontal cortex. This dual function might, at least in part, explain the opposite effect of the mutation on reference memory (facilitation) and delay-dependent working memory (impairment). These results support the idea that cholinergic-serotonergic interactions play an important role in memory processes. 相似文献