首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   590篇
  免费   24篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   5篇
  2021年   17篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   16篇
  2017年   20篇
  2016年   14篇
  2015年   8篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   65篇
  2012年   20篇
  2011年   27篇
  2010年   13篇
  2009年   20篇
  2008年   25篇
  2007年   26篇
  2006年   23篇
  2005年   32篇
  2004年   18篇
  2003年   32篇
  2002年   28篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   11篇
  1999年   11篇
  1998年   13篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   8篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   3篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   2篇
  1975年   4篇
  1972年   2篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
排序方式: 共有614条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
461.
462.
463.
Isolating Unconscious Influences: The Neutral Parameter Procedure   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Earlier demonstrations of unconscious learning have been regularly challenged. In this paper, we suggest that earlier demonstrations were not compelling due to certain properties of the experimental situations, and notably that conscious exploitation of explicit knowledge, if present, would coincide or conflict with the results of unconscious processing. We designed a method consisting of inducing a neutral behavioural change in the way subjects drew geometric figures. Two experiments showed that important and long-lasting modifications of drawing behaviour were obtained following specially devised practice, although these modifications could not be expected from deliberate adaptive strategies. In addition, we showed that subjects were unaware of the manipulation to which they had been exposed. The study provides striking evidence for unconscious learning and offers insights for the design of suitable new tools to investigate unconscious cognition.  相似文献   
464.
465.
466.
A risk group of disruptive boys (N=65) identified in kindergarten was assessed using the same procedures at ages 6–7, 8–9, and 10–11. Criteria used to define the predictors and criterion variable were the same at all assessment times. Severity was addressed by comparing different forms of behaviors, considering the extent of harmful consequences to others (aggressiveness was considered as most harmful, whereas inattention was considered as least harmful), manifestation in multiple settings, and extreme scores (manipulation of cutoff scores). Different assessment strategies (direct observations, ratings) and different informants (trained observers, mothers and teachers) were used. Aggressiveness as rated by mothers was highly stable from age 6 to age 11. Inattention as rated by teachers was stable only from age 6 to age 9, whereas no stability was found for observations of task inappropriate behaviors. Taskinappropriate behaviors observed in mother-child interactions and in multiple settings at age 6–7 were significant predictors of self-reported antisocial behaviors at age 12, but this prediction was not repeated at ages 8–9 and 10–11. Teacher ratings of inattention at ages 6–7 and 8–9 were also significant predictors of self-reported antisocial behaviors at age 12. The predictive power was much lower when mothers' ratings of aggressiveness were used. Findings from the present study support the hypothesis that some antisocial behavior precursors are age dependent, in that they are more characteristic of certain age groups than of others. Implications for the selection of assessment screening procedures are discussed.  相似文献   
467.
Pierre Duhem 《Synthese》1990,83(2):183-188
Part II of Duhem 1917, pp. 151–57, translated by Peter Barker and Roger Ariew; published also by Duhem in Duhem 1913a, pp. 737–40.  相似文献   
468.
In a recent experiment (Lewicki et al., 1988) subjects were submitted to a four-choice RT paradigm for 3600 trials. On each of the successive logical blocks of five trials, the first two locations of the target were randomly distributed, and the last three locations were determined by complex rules. Although subjects were unable to verbalize the actual nature of the manipulation, performance on the last trials of each block improved at a faster rate and was better overall than performance on the first trials. In addition, subsequent rules changes on 480 additional trials only affected performance on the last three trials of each block. The present paper demonstrates that contrary to Lewicki et al's assertions this performance pattern requires neither acquisition of tacit knowledge of the composition rules, nor partitioning by the subjects of the sequence into logical blocks of five trials. Rather, the results can be accounted for by the relative frequency of a few simple sequences of target locations. Moreover, this alternative explanation alone correctly anticipates some striking features of fine-grained performance (Lewicki et al., 1988). The discussion focuses on methodological implications of these findings for investigation of unconscious learning, and speculates on what and how people learn when they encounter a complex and structured situation.  相似文献   
469.
It is possible to consider attention as the modulating dimension of consciousness. Understood in this sense, attention can be a privileged theme for relating the first person point of view (conceived as a psycho-phenomenology inspired by the work of Husserl) to the experimental sciences (e.g. psychology, neuropsychology, etc.), which have done a great deal of work on attention. This article will take up in succession some different points of view regarding the status of attention and its structure (e.g. static aspects). It will also consider the dynamic of attention from a micro-genetic point of view as well as a functional point of view. The final section will seek to show not only the unique and original contributions of each perspective, but also each perspective's limitations and biases.  相似文献   
470.
In three experiments, we addressed the role of stereotypes in the attribution of action tendencies in intergroup contexts. We hypothesized that stereotyping would affect the attribution of action tendencies to out‐group members. Participants were presented with a facial expression displayed by either an in‐group or an out‐group member, followed by the presentation of a label describing an action tendency. They were then asked whether the label corresponded to the feeling state of the expresser. Study 1 tested whether stereotypes influence the attribution of action tendencies to out‐group members. Study 2 tested whether stereotype application varies as a function of the emotional information contained in the facial stimuli (i.e. neutral vs. emotional). Finally, Study 3 tested whether stereotype activation is indirectly determined by a difference in morphology between in‐group and out‐group members or directly determined by the expresser's group membership. As predicted, an increase in attribution of stereotypic action tendencies was observed for out‐group expressers. The application of stereotypes was specifically observed when facial expressions were neutral as compared to emotional and was independent of morphological differences between in‐group and out‐group faces. Such biases in interpreting out‐group members feeling states may play a crucial role in the maintenance of intergroup prejudice. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号