全文获取类型
收费全文 | 227篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
227篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 22篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 29篇 |
2012年 | 18篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 4篇 |
2007年 | 6篇 |
2006年 | 3篇 |
2005年 | 2篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 2篇 |
1955年 | 2篇 |
1949年 | 1篇 |
1948年 | 1篇 |
1947年 | 1篇 |
1937年 | 2篇 |
1936年 | 3篇 |
1935年 | 3篇 |
1934年 | 1篇 |
1930年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有227条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Dipl.-Psych. Amelie Thobaben Dr. Dipl.-Psych. Philipp Soldt 《Forum der Psychoanalyse》2007,23(4):330-342
Following the recent development of special treatment for people with defective ego structures, the diagnosis of ego structures has become highly relevant. Four models, developed during the last 40 years for the differentiation of particular levels of the ego structure are presented: the classification of character pathology (Kernberg 1966), the personality organisations (Kernberg 1977 and 1996), the structure axis of the OPD (Operationalized Psychodynamic Diagnosis 1996) and the personality organisations of the STIPO (2006). Because the levels of character pathology, the personality organisations as well as the structural levels of the OPD are widely used and the last two often even in parallel, it is worthwhile comparing them concerning their strengths, weaknesses and compatibility. It turns out that especially the categories between the neurotic and the borderline personality organisation (Kernberg 1977) show significant differences. The fact that the OPD as well as the STIPO pay much more attention to these levels as the models of personality organisations also do, is to be considered because of its great clinical significance as an advantageous innovation. In addition, both instruments offer the possibility to assign different structural levels to particular dimensions and, due to their operationalization, assure comparability and allow change measurement. 相似文献
62.
Research on the association between personality traits and driving behavior as well as accident involvement has produced mixed results. In order to obtain empirical evidence for a consistent relation between personality and driving behavior, a prototype approach based on the Five-Factor Model (FFM) was applied. In two samples of drivers, cluster analyses of FFM scales revealed three prototypes that replicate previous ones. The prototypes, labeled Resilient, Overcontrolled, and Undercontrolled, were found to differ reliably in accident involvement and driving behavior, indicating differential accident liability. Undercontrollers are the most problematic drivers followed by Resilients, whereas Overcontrollers most consistently obey traffic rules and drive accordingly. 相似文献
63.
Lars Lewejohann Anne Marie Hoppmann Philipp Kegel Mareike Kritzler Antonio Krüger Norbert Sachser 《Behavior research methods》2009,41(3):850-856
Neurodegen erative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) are increasingly threatening public health. Most animal models
of AD consist of transgenic mice that are usually housed singly or in unisexual groups in small barren cages. Such restricted
environments, however, prevent the mice from showing a variety of speciesspecific behaviors and consequently may constrain
comprehensive behavioral phenotyping. On the other hand, allowing the animals to freely organize their lives in a spacious
physically and socially enriched environment makes behavioral phenotyping laborious and time consuming. Radio frequency identification
(RFID) using a network of antennae and small glass-coated transponders labeling each individual allows for gathering spatiotemporal
information about a large number of individuals in parallel. The aim of this project was to use the RFID technique to facilitate
the characterization of mice carrying a genetic disposition to develop AD-like pathology and of their wild-type conspecifics
in a spacious seminaturalistic environment. 相似文献
64.
Research has confirmed a healthy link between young adults’ religiosity and body image. This study explored this relationship
in 127 older men and women (mean = 74 years) who completed measures on two dimensions of body image (body satisfaction and
anxiety about an aging appearance) and on different indices of religiosity. Men reported higher body satisfaction and lower
anxiety about an aging appearance than women. For men, body satisfaction correlated with many religiosity variables and was
predicted by religious well-being, existential well-being, and manifestation of God in their body; aging-appearance anxiety
was unrelated to religiosity. For women, body satisfaction was weakly related to religiosity but aging-appearance anxiety
was predicted by intrinsic orientation, religious well-being, and existential well-being; in all cases higher religiosity
predicted lower anxiety about an aging appearance. Results are discussed in the context of differing cultural standards of
aging and attractiveness for men and women. 相似文献
65.
66.
67.
68.
Philipp Frank 《Erkenntnis》1930,1(1):126-157
Ohne Zusammenfassung
Vortrag, gehalten in der Er?ffnungssitzung des deutschen Physiker-und Mathematikertages in Prag am 16. September 1929. 相似文献
69.
A task switch typically leads to worse performance than a repetition does. This shift cost can be reduced with sufficient task preparation time, but a residual cost usually remains. We propose that a large part of this residual cost is caused by an activation bias produced by response selection processes in the preceding trial. In our experiments, we manipulated response selection requirements using a go/no-go methodology. The residual shift cost disappeared after no-go trials, suggesting that response selection is crucial to establish an activation bias for the current category-response rules and that this bias persists into the next trial. A comparison with a go-only group confirmed this analysis by revealing no differences in preparatory strategy due to the inclusion of no-go trials. In addition, no-go trials had no significant effects on subsequent trials in a single-task experiment, suggesting that no-go trials are not coded as a task different from go trials and that there is no inhibition of the prepared task in a no-go trial. We thus conclude that a persisting activation bias of response rules plays a major role in task switching. 相似文献
70.