首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   26665篇
  免费   966篇
  国内免费   12篇
  2018年   376篇
  2017年   406篇
  2016年   413篇
  2015年   292篇
  2014年   379篇
  2013年   2009篇
  2012年   658篇
  2011年   718篇
  2010年   490篇
  2009年   445篇
  2008年   685篇
  2007年   639篇
  2006年   594篇
  2005年   526篇
  2004年   524篇
  2003年   518篇
  2002年   543篇
  2001年   669篇
  2000年   674篇
  1999年   531篇
  1998年   287篇
  1997年   300篇
  1996年   300篇
  1995年   271篇
  1992年   512篇
  1991年   467篇
  1990年   462篇
  1989年   433篇
  1988年   451篇
  1987年   420篇
  1986年   468篇
  1985年   445篇
  1984年   403篇
  1983年   356篇
  1982年   277篇
  1981年   292篇
  1979年   422篇
  1978年   349篇
  1977年   304篇
  1976年   312篇
  1975年   351篇
  1974年   447篇
  1973年   483篇
  1972年   351篇
  1971年   373篇
  1970年   340篇
  1969年   366篇
  1968年   440篇
  1967年   408篇
  1966年   417篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
12.
13.
We investigated motivational and cognitive processes of behavior change with respect to mammography screening. One hundred forty-two women (ages 40 and older) recruited from three worksites answered a 41-item questionnaire consisting of statements based on constructs from the transtheoretical model of behavior change. Principal-components analysis identified two factors: a six-item component representing positive perceptions of mammography (Pros) and a six-item component representing avoidance of mammography (Cons). Analysis of variance showed that Pros, Cons, and a derived Decisional Balance measure (Pros minus Cons) were associated with stage of mammography adoption. Results are consistent with applications of the model to smoking cessation. The model is also discussed as it relates to other theories of behavior change and as a general strategy for analyzing perceptual data pertinent to health-related actions and intentions for behavioral change.  相似文献   
14.
15.
16.
We investigated eye‐movements during preschool children's pictorial recall of seen objects. Thirteen 3‐ to 4‐year‐old children completed a perceptual encoding and a pictorial recall task. First, they were exposed to 16 pictorial objects, which were positioned in one of four distinct areas on the computer screen. Subsequently, they had to recall these pictorial objects from memory in order to respond to specific questions about visual details. We found that children spent more time fixating the areas in which the pictorial objects were previously displayed. We conclude that as early as age 3–4 years old, children show specific eye‐movements when they recall pictorial contents of previously seen objects.  相似文献   
17.
18.
We discuss computational language analysis as it pertains to suicide prevention research, with an emphasis on providing non‐technologists with an understanding of key issues and, equally important, considering its relation to the broader enterprise of suicide prevention. Our emphasis here is on naturally occurring language in social media, motivated by its non‐intrusive ability to yield high‐value information that in the past has been largely unavailable to clinicians.  相似文献   
19.
20.
Two studies tested the hypothesis that certain positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. In Study 1, 60 subjects (Ss) viewed an initial fear-eliciting film, and were randomly assigned to view a secondary film that elicited: (a) contentment; (b) amusement; (c) neutrality; or (d) sadness. Compared to Ss who viewed the neutral and sad secondary films, those who viewed the positive films exhibited more rapid returns to pre-film levels of cardiovascular activation. In Study 2, 72 Ss viewed a film known to elicit sadness. Fifty Ss spontaneously smiled at least once while viewing this film. Compared to Ss who did not smile, those who smiled exhibited more rapid returns to pre-film levels of cardiovascular activation. We discuss these findings in terms of emotion theory and possible health-promoting functions of positive emotions.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号