全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2205篇 |
免费 | 70篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
2278篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 34篇 |
2019年 | 40篇 |
2018年 | 53篇 |
2017年 | 59篇 |
2016年 | 57篇 |
2015年 | 37篇 |
2014年 | 50篇 |
2013年 | 274篇 |
2012年 | 89篇 |
2011年 | 93篇 |
2010年 | 65篇 |
2009年 | 58篇 |
2008年 | 76篇 |
2007年 | 103篇 |
2006年 | 72篇 |
2005年 | 77篇 |
2004年 | 71篇 |
2003年 | 59篇 |
2002年 | 81篇 |
2001年 | 35篇 |
2000年 | 39篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 36篇 |
1997年 | 32篇 |
1996年 | 31篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 29篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 28篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 14篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 28篇 |
1983年 | 25篇 |
1982年 | 31篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 26篇 |
1979年 | 22篇 |
1978年 | 29篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 21篇 |
1975年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 20篇 |
1973年 | 23篇 |
1967年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有2278条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
Scientific Discovery A. Kantorovich 1993 New York, State University of New York Press $17.95
A Model of the Universe Storrs McCall, 1994 Clarendon Press, Oxford 288pp. plus 15pp. appendices, plus 14pp. references plus index 0198240538 £30.00
Explanation David Hillel Ruben (ed. 1993) Oxford University Press pp vi + 365 ISBN 019875129X. Pb £9.95, Hb £27.50 相似文献
32.
Philip Hefner 《Zygon》1994,29(1):67-73
Abstract. This piece challenges Michael Ruse on three points: (1) The charge that Christian myth and doctrine are incredible fails to take into account the scholarship that has clarified the genre to which myth belongs and its function. (2) Naturalistic explanations, like Ruse's, have fully as much difficulty dealing with questions of purpose and evil as religion does. (3) The concept of "deception" has a number of inherent problems that Ruse fails to consider, of which the chief is that it requires a certainty about truth and falsity that Ruse cannot and does not claim to possess. 相似文献
33.
34.
35.
Philip N. Johnson-Laird 《Cognition》1994,50(1-3):189-209
This paper outlines the theory of reasoning based on mental models, and then shows how this theory might be extended to deal with probabilistic thinking. The same explanatory framework accommodates deduction and induction: there are both deductive and inductive inferences that yield probabilistic conclusions. The framework yields a theoretical conception of strength of inference, that is, a theory of what the strength of an inference is objectively: it equals the proportion of possible states of affairs consistent with the premises in which the conclusion is true, that is, the probability that the conclusion is true given that the premises are true. Since there are infinitely many possible states of affairs consistent with any set of premises, the paper then characterizes how individuals estimate the strength of an argument. They construct mental models, which each correspond to an infinite set of possibilities (or, in some cases, a finite set of infinite sets of possibilities). The construction of models is guided by knowledge and beliefs, including lay conceptions of such matters as the “law of large numbers”. The paper illustrates how this theory can account for phenomena of probabilistic reasoning. 相似文献
36.
The distribution of males and females on a job, occupational classification, and job content as defined by the DOT analysis of job activity demands were examined as predictors of job sex stereotypes in two studies. Results indicate that the actual base rate of males and females in the job is the most important predictor of job sextypes, accounting for 48 and 70% of the adjusted variance in sextypes in the two studies. The results lend little support to the proposition that job content makes a significant contribution to the perception of job sextypes. 相似文献
37.
38.
Human subjects learned a key-pressing response in order to avoid or escape shock. The reinforcement contingencies were then changed to punishment or to regular nonpunished extinction. The locus of shock onset and offset was systematically varied during the punishment phase. More subjects reported awarencess of the change in the nonpunished extinction group. By inference, the punished groups responded more, and thus the results appear analogous to animal studies on vicious circle, self-punitive responding. Discriminability of change from acquisition to extinction appeared to affect detection of the change. 相似文献
39.
Philip H. K. Seymour Maureen V. Jack 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1978,30(3):455-469
An experiment was conducted in which subjects matched upper and lower case versions of well-known abbreviations, such as BBC and etc, and meaningless controls. “Same” RT showed a familiarity effect for upper case versions of abbreviations such as BBC and GPO, but not for the lower case versions bbc and gpo. The converse did not occur for abbreviations such as etc, which were thought to occur most frequently in lower case. The “different” RT was inhibited by familiarity, with pairs such as IBM GPO being classified less rapidly than their lower case versions or controls. These effects occurred for subjects instructed to report “No” for “same” displays and “Yes” for “different” displays as well as for subjects given a conventional decision-report assignment. Some implications of these results for an account of the manner in which familiarity affects graphemic comparison processes are considered. 相似文献
40.
N.H Azrin R.A Philip P Thienes-Hontos V.A Besalel 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1980,16(2):133-145
About 1000 WIN welfare clients were selected in five cities: Harlem, New Brunswick, Milwaukee, Wichita, and Tacoma, and half were randomly assigned to the Job Club program. Of the continuing clients, 87% of the Job Club sample obtained jobs vs 59% of the Control sample at the 12-month follow-up and 80 vs 48% at 6 months. The Job Club was more effective in each of the five cities, for men and women, for high school graduates or dropouts, for blacks, whites, and Spanish, for handicapped or nonhandicapped, veterans or nonveterans, the young and the older, and for those required to participate as well as those who volunteered. The jobs obtained by the Job Club clients were comparable to the Control clients' jobs in terms of mean salary, full-time status, and type of job, and were more likely to be enduring, nonsubsidized, and obtained by the job-seeker's own efforts. Job Club members obtained employment in a median of six sessions (mean of 11); 90% obtained jobs within 23 sessions. Follow-up questionnaire data indicated comparable job retention in the two samples but slightly greater advancement and job satisfaction for the Job Club clients. The method appears to assure employment to virtually all participating welfare clients. 相似文献