首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   103111篇
  免费   4608篇
  国内免费   74篇
  107793篇
  2020年   1048篇
  2019年   1324篇
  2018年   1767篇
  2017年   1831篇
  2016年   1974篇
  2015年   1408篇
  2014年   1688篇
  2013年   7996篇
  2012年   3147篇
  2011年   3360篇
  2010年   2151篇
  2009年   2174篇
  2008年   2971篇
  2007年   2972篇
  2006年   2742篇
  2005年   2490篇
  2004年   2342篇
  2003年   2256篇
  2002年   2306篇
  2001年   3127篇
  2000年   2999篇
  1999年   2345篇
  1998年   1231篇
  1997年   1115篇
  1996年   1120篇
  1993年   979篇
  1992年   2003篇
  1991年   1898篇
  1990年   1880篇
  1989年   1699篇
  1988年   1648篇
  1987年   1588篇
  1986年   1691篇
  1985年   1773篇
  1984年   1497篇
  1983年   1353篇
  1982年   1008篇
  1981年   987篇
  1979年   1542篇
  1978年   1130篇
  1975年   1297篇
  1974年   1458篇
  1973年   1531篇
  1972年   1312篇
  1971年   1224篇
  1970年   1114篇
  1969年   1137篇
  1968年   1488篇
  1967年   1326篇
  1966年   1170篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
52.
53.
54.
55.
An electronic apparatus using infrared beams for monitoring the movements of individual bees under dark conditions is described. The searching behavior of workers in an arena was monitored over 2-h periods. Mean ambulatory velocity for one bee over a distance of 100 mm was 45.6±1.51 mm/sec (n=45). Thigmokinesis and temporal activity patterns are illustrated.  相似文献   
56.
57.
58.
Previous work on children's intuitive knowledge about the natural world has documented their difficulty in acquiring an overarching concept of biological life that includes plants as well as humans and non‐human animals. It has also suggested that the acquisition of fundamental biological concepts like alive and die may be influenced by the language used to describe them, as evidenced by differences between English‐ and Indonesian‐speaking children's performance in tasks involving these concepts. Here, we examine one particularly important source of linguistic information available to children during this acquisition process: everyday conversations with their parents. We take a cross‐linguistic approach in analysing the evidence available to English‐ and Indonesian‐speaking children as they acquire meanings for words corresponding to the concepts alive and die . Our analysis illustrates that young children acquiring English and Indonesian are faced with distinct problems, but that parental input in both languages does little to support the acquisition of broad, inclusive biological concepts.  相似文献   
59.
The ability to make egocentric distance estimates of a single point source of light, seen in darkness and without the cues of changing size and luminance, was investigated in sixteen observers. The attenuation required to maintain constant luminance, when the target was viewed from different distances, was shown to follow the inverse square law providing the angle subtended by the light was less than 20 s arc. Distance changes were also simulated by means of a split mirror which produced vergence cues, or by test lenses to provide accommodation cues. Over the range 0.5 to 9.2 m distance estimates were surprisingly accurate, although there was some overestimation of near and underestimation of far distances. Most observers made good judgements when only convergence cues were varied, whereas no observers made consistently good judgements when only accommodation cues were varied. The difficulties are discussed in terms of the accommodation-convergence link. When distance was simulated by changing convergence and accommodation cues, estimates were not as good as when real distance was changed. Since good estimates were made with brief target exposures, these judgements were not based on subsequent convergence or accommodation changes. It is suggested that the metric or reference against which the apparently absolute judgements were made was the efferent demand signal associated with a 'resting' position of convergence in darkness.  相似文献   
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号