首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2258篇
  免费   71篇
  国内免费   4篇
  2333篇
  2021年   11篇
  2020年   35篇
  2019年   41篇
  2018年   53篇
  2017年   62篇
  2016年   58篇
  2015年   37篇
  2014年   51篇
  2013年   282篇
  2012年   93篇
  2011年   99篇
  2010年   67篇
  2009年   58篇
  2008年   78篇
  2007年   109篇
  2006年   75篇
  2005年   77篇
  2004年   72篇
  2003年   59篇
  2002年   84篇
  2001年   36篇
  2000年   39篇
  1999年   32篇
  1998年   36篇
  1997年   32篇
  1996年   31篇
  1995年   19篇
  1994年   29篇
  1993年   25篇
  1992年   29篇
  1991年   22篇
  1990年   19篇
  1989年   17篇
  1988年   14篇
  1987年   15篇
  1986年   21篇
  1985年   17篇
  1984年   28篇
  1983年   25篇
  1982年   33篇
  1981年   30篇
  1980年   26篇
  1979年   22篇
  1978年   29篇
  1977年   24篇
  1976年   21篇
  1975年   18篇
  1974年   20篇
  1973年   25篇
  1967年   12篇
排序方式: 共有2333条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
Subjects from Grades 3, 4, 6, and college judged whether pairs of stimuli were identical or mirror-image reversals. One stimulus of a pair was presented upright; the other was rotated 0 to 150° from the standard. The pairs were either alphanumeric symbols or unfamiliar, letter-like characters of the type found on the PMA Spatial Ability Test. Response latencies were measured. The primary results were that (a) the speed of mental rotation increased with development, (b) unfamiliar characters were rotated more slowly than alphanumeric characters, by approximately the same amount at each grade, and (c) unfamiliar characters were encoded and compared more slowly than alphanumeric symbols, by an amount that declined with development. The results are discussed in terms of the component processes that underlie developmental change and individual variation in mental rotation skill.  相似文献   
962.
Sixty-four infants, eight boys and eight girls at each of four ages, 912, 1112, 1312, and 1512 months, were observed during a 15-minute free play session. Three types of play (stereotypical, relational, and functional) and the number of appropriate uses for specific realistic toys were recorded. Stereotypical play (87% of total active play at 912 months) gave way to relational (35% at 1312 months) and functional play (52% at 1512 months) as the dominant activity. Both functional play and the number of different appropriate uses of toys were rare at 912 months, appeared reliably at 1112 months and increased lineraly through 1512 months. A change in the quality of play from indiscriminate mouthing, waving, banging, and fingering of objects to the matching of appropriate uses for a large array of toys indicates that the child's manipulations become more object specific and functional over this 6-month age span. Among possible explanations for the linear increase in the frequency and diversity of appropriate toy uses is the suggestion that an important cognitive change occurs toward the end of the first year of life. A case study is presented to illustrate the application of the play procedure for both assessment and treatment.  相似文献   
963.
Subjects classified sets of two-dimensional stimuli into two or three groups in any way they wished. For integral stimuli, classification was based on numerical balance and the similarity structure. For separable stimuli, classification was based on the dimensional structure and the configurai properties of the stimulus set. These results replicate and extend those of Handel and Imai (1972). In addition, they suggest that no closed set of stimulus concepts is possible.  相似文献   
964.
Milgram et al. (1972) proposed the formula R = f(C X D) for the recognition of city scenes where R = Recognition, C = Centrality to the population flow and D = architectural or social distinctiveness. In this paper a slight modification to this formula was suggested. The centrality concept was replaced by the more adequate one of familiarity and an empirical test of the proposition was carried out. Fifty-four scenes of urban Adelaide, South Australia were chosen using the two techniques of fixed distance sampling along major roads and equivalent representation sampling from the entire urban area. High School students (N = 41) and University Students (N = 31) were shown colour slides and ratings were obtained for recognition and distinctiveness. Familiarity was assessed using frequency of travel patterns. Multiple regression analyses showed that frequency of encounter was the only significant predictor variable in the younger High School sample, but distinctiveness and frequency were both significant for the University sample. A possible explanation for this difference in terms of car usage is outlined and some critical comments on the distinctiveness concept are presented.  相似文献   
965.
Chambers  Clare  Cook  Philip  Lægaard  Sune 《Res Publica》2020,26(2):155-156
  相似文献   
966.
967.
968.
969.
A survey was sent to doctoral school psychologists to determine their research productivity and topical interests. Approximately two thirds of 147 respondents indicated doing some research during 1974–1975. Studies dealing with topics of assessment and program evaluation were most prevalent. Only about one in five studies was actually published and most of these were in nonprestigious periodicals. Academic school psychologists were more likely to do research and publish than were applied school psychologists. Academic and applied psychologists did not differ in terms of numbers of unpublished written or oral research reports. The correspondence of these results with prior data on school psychology research was noted and suggestions to increase the circulation of research findings are presented.  相似文献   
970.
The physician-patient relationship has critical importance in the quality of medical care, especially in the oncology setting. Of particular interest over the past two decades has been the communication between physicians and their patients. It is understood that communication is related to patient and physician satisfaction as well as other medical outcomes. However, despite this interest and attention, investigators have only recently begun to assess the communication between physicians and patients from the patient's point of view. Additionally, previous investigations have primarily relied upon retrospective reports of the interaction from patients or physicians. The current investigation assessed physician–patient communication in an oncology setting, both as it was occurring and following the interaction. The units of analysis included participant ratings as well as those of trained observers. Findings revealed that physicians and patients disagree as to the valence of the messages which transpired. Additionally, results suggest that it is the patients who distort the messages in the positive direction. Research and clinical implications of this disagreement and positivity bias are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号