全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2201篇 |
免费 | 70篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
2274篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 34篇 |
2019年 | 40篇 |
2018年 | 53篇 |
2017年 | 59篇 |
2016年 | 57篇 |
2015年 | 37篇 |
2014年 | 50篇 |
2013年 | 274篇 |
2012年 | 89篇 |
2011年 | 93篇 |
2010年 | 65篇 |
2009年 | 58篇 |
2008年 | 76篇 |
2007年 | 103篇 |
2006年 | 72篇 |
2005年 | 77篇 |
2004年 | 70篇 |
2003年 | 59篇 |
2002年 | 81篇 |
2001年 | 35篇 |
2000年 | 38篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 35篇 |
1997年 | 32篇 |
1996年 | 31篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 29篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 28篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1988年 | 14篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 28篇 |
1983年 | 25篇 |
1982年 | 31篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 26篇 |
1979年 | 22篇 |
1978年 | 29篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 21篇 |
1975年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 20篇 |
1973年 | 23篇 |
1967年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有2274条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
The generality and stability of Holland's vocational choice model was tested by examining preference for avocational (leisure) activity. Based on their responses to the Strong Vocational Interest Blank, college students were placed into one of the six Holland groups. They also indicated preferred leisure activities on questionnaires that were administered at a 9-year interval. Six hypotheses were developed relating Holland groups to particular types of avocational behavior. For example, it was predicted that the Artistic group would be more active in cultural affairs and that the Enterprising group would prefer reading political and economic magazines. The initial test of these hypotheses during late adolescence confirmed five of the six predictions. A follow-up 9 years later confirmed all six hypotheses. These results provide some support for the notion that individuals self-select a variety of situations. Also, the relationships between Holland scales and leisure activity and possible implications for the vocational choice model are discussed. 相似文献
52.
Two studies were conducted to test the hypothesis that exposure to non-contingent escape leads to performance deficits similar to those observed when subjects are exposed to noncontingent aversive outcomes from which there is no escape, and that causal attributions mediate these deficits. Previous attempts to produce “appetitive helplessness” (deficits resulting from exposure to noncontingent positive events) have been plagued by subjects' tendency to believe that they are responsible for positive events. In Experiment 1, 40 subjects were exposed to contingent or noncontingent noise escape trials. As predicted by the learned helplessness model, subjects who received inescapable noise performed less well on a subsequent anagram task than subjects exposed to escapable noise. Similarly, subjects who escaped from the noise owing to the benevolence of a powerful other rather than because of their own efforts, showed performance deficits paralleling those of the inescapable noise subjects. In Experiment 2, subjects who escaped an aversive tone through no effort of their own showed subsequent performance deficits, but globality of their self-reported attributions did not predict subsequent anagram performance. The results of these studies provide support for the hypothesis that uncontrollability, independent of the valence of a particular outcome, is responsible for helplessness deficits, but do not support the mediational role of attributions, at least in the laboratory. 相似文献
53.
Proctor (1981) presented a theoretical framework that distinguishes factors contributing to disparities in time to classify physical-same, name-same, and different letter pairs as a function of three variables: method of presentation (simultaneous vs. successive), case relationship (samecase vs. opposite-case pairs), and blocking (blocked vs. mixed presentation of same-case and opposite-case pairs). He also argued that these variables were critical in multIletter matches, and performed a between-study comparison of existing data to support his contention. Because comparison across studies is always a tenuous process and because the absence of several relevant conditions precluded a complete analysis of predictions, a within-experiment manipulation of the three relevant variables was desirable. The present study reports such an experiment. In general, the factorial manipulation of variables supported predictions of Proctor’s framework and indicated that many phenomena of multiletter matching, including the widely studied fast-“same” phenomenon, are attributable primarily to differences in the rate at which component letter pairs are matched. 相似文献
54.
Donald R Goodenough Philip K Oltman Florence Friedman Carol Ann Moore Herman A Witkin David Owen Evelyn Raskin 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1979,14(3):341-351
Recent research has shown that academic choice and achievement may be partly a function of the student's standing on the field-dependence-independence cognitive-style dimension. The results of two longitudinal studies suggest that information about field dependence-independence may be of value for student guidance in the medical setting. The first, a study of college undergraduates who early expressed an interest in medicine, showed that these cognitive styles play a discernible role in determining who will eventually enter medical school. The second, a study of medical students, showed that field-dependent and field-independent students subsequently tend to choose different medical specialties. These results are consistent with cognitive-style theory, which proposes that field-independent people will choose vocations that require cognitive restructuring skills, whereas field-dependent people will choose vocations that require greater social-interpersonal involvement. 相似文献
55.
56.
THOG: The anatomy of a problem 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Summary Three experiments are reported on the attempts to solve a novel hypothetico-deductive problem. Its solution demands both the postulation of hypotheses about its structure and a combinatorial analysis upon the consequences of these hypotheses. The majority of subjects (students) failed to solve the problem because they argued from the properties of stimuli rather than from hypotheses about their conceptual status. The results suggest that a familiarity with the logical structure of the problem and the elicitation of appropriate hypotheses failed to correct this intuitive approach. These findings are discussed in relation to Piaget's theory of formal operations, and (very tentatively) in relation to habitual styles of thought. 相似文献
57.
58.
Philip H. K. Seymour 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1977,29(2):245-265
Subjects reacted to coloured season and month names by naming the season associated with the print colour of the word, or by naming the season opposite to the one associated with the print colour. Reactions were facilitated in both tasks when the word on the display named the season associated with the colour or was a month belonging to that season. Opposite season naming was not facilitated when the word named the response which the subject made. A comparable effect of print colour was found when subjects reacted to season names by naming the colour associated with the name, or with its opposite. These results suggest that Stroop congruity and interference effects occur during conceptual encoding, and not during response production. 相似文献
59.
Philip F. Spelt 《Behavior research methods》1977,9(2):87-91
A 20-day computer conference was held in preparation for a face-to-face meeting to produce a computer simulation. The conferencing resulted in the generation of seven different models and was excellent preparation for the subsequent meeting. Computer conferencing appears to hold great value for faculty development in a period of reduced faculty mobility. 相似文献
60.
Philip Smith Michel Treisman M. J. A. Simpson M. Burton F. J. McGuigan 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1977,29(4):745-749
Wyer, Robert S., Jr. Cognitive Organization and Change : an Information Processing Approach. Potomac, Maryland: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1974. Pp. 502. £10.50.
Egan, J. P. Signal Detection Theory and ROC Analysis. New York: Academic Press. 1975. Pp. 277. £9.75. ISBN
Aldis, O. Play-Fighting. New York: Academic Press. 1975. Pp. 319. £8.75.
Warburton, D. M. Brain Behaviour and Drugs. London: John Wiley. 1975. Pp. 280. £6.95. ISBN 0471 91991 8.
Grieve, D. W., Miller, D., Mitchelson, D., Paul, J. and Smith, A. J. Techniques for the Analysis of Human Movement. London: Lepus Books. 1975. Pp. 177. £5.50. 相似文献
Egan, J. P. Signal Detection Theory and ROC Analysis. New York: Academic Press. 1975. Pp. 277. £9.75. ISBN
Aldis, O. Play-Fighting. New York: Academic Press. 1975. Pp. 319. £8.75.
Warburton, D. M. Brain Behaviour and Drugs. London: John Wiley. 1975. Pp. 280. £6.95. ISBN 0471 91991 8.
Grieve, D. W., Miller, D., Mitchelson, D., Paul, J. and Smith, A. J. Techniques for the Analysis of Human Movement. London: Lepus Books. 1975. Pp. 177. £5.50. 相似文献