全文获取类型
收费全文 | 818篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
833篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 14篇 |
2017年 | 19篇 |
2016年 | 28篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 78篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 29篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 25篇 |
2008年 | 28篇 |
2007年 | 23篇 |
2006年 | 25篇 |
2005年 | 14篇 |
2004年 | 30篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 20篇 |
1999年 | 16篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 12篇 |
1967年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
1965年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有833条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
321.
Peterson MA Skow E 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2008,34(2):251-267
Theories of figure-ground perception entail inhibitory competition between either low-level units (edge or feature units) or high-level shape properties. Extant computational models instantiate the 1st type of theory. The authors investigated a prediction of the 2nd type of theory: that shape properties suggested on the ground side of an edge are suppressed when they lose the figure-ground competition. In Experiment 1, the authors present behavioral evidence of the predicted suppression: Object decisions were slower for line drawings that followed silhouettes suggesting portions of objects from the same rather than a different category on their ground sides. In Experiment 2, the authors reversed the silhouette's figure-ground relationships and obtained speeding rather than slowing in the same category condition, thereby demonstrating that the Experiment 1 results reflect suppression of those shape properties that lose the figure-ground competition. These experiments provide the first clear empirical evidence that figure-ground perception entails inhibitory competition between high-level shape properties and demonstrate the need for amendments to existing computational models. Furthermore, these results suggest that figure-ground perception may itself be an instance of biased competition in shape perception. 相似文献
322.
This research reports an investigation of the use of standardized regression (beta) coefficients in meta-analyses that use correlation coefficients as the effect-size metric. The investigation consisted of analyzing more than 1,700 corresponding beta coefficients and correlation coefficients harvested from published studies. Results indicate that, under certain conditions, using knowledge of corresponding beta coefficients to input missing correlations (effect sizes) generally produces relatively accurate and precise population effect-size estimates. Potential benefits from applying this knowledge include smaller sampling errors because of increased numbers of effect sizes and smaller non-sampling errors because of the inclusion of a broader array of research designs. 相似文献
323.
Martin Peterson 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2010,13(2):153-169
To consequentialise a moral theory means to account for moral phenomena usually described in nonconsequentialist terms, such
as rights, duties, and virtues, in a consequentialist framework. This paper seeks to show that all moral theories can be consequentialised.
The paper distinguishes between different interpretations of the consequentialiser’s thesis, and emphasises the need for a
cardinal ranking of acts. The paper also offers a new answer as to why consequentialising moral theories is important: This
yields crucial methodological insights about how to pursue ethical inquires. 相似文献
324.
After reviewing literature detailing the ubiquity of error in research and assessment data, I describe mistakes found with MMPI-2 and Rorschach scores in an earlier publication (Meyer, 1993). The mistakes emerged from several sources, including hand-scoring the assessment measures, manually retrieving scores from patient files, and manually entering information for statistical analyses. The proportion of erroneous scores in the original study (MMPI-2 = 5.88%, Rorschach = 1.56%) was relatively small and reliability coefficients between the original and corrected scales were uniformly high (i.e., > .940). Consequently, the findings detailed in the original publication were not greatly affected by the mistakes that were made. Nonetheless, because this will not always be the case, I discuss the discrepancies as a way to sensitize researchers, clinicians, and students to the presence of error and then suggest several strategies for minimizing its impact on assessment data and research. 相似文献
325.
Carole. Peterson Duyen T. K. Nguyen 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2010,101(4):719-737
The first years of life are typically shrouded by infantile amnesia, but there is enormous variability between adults in how early and how much they can remember from this period. This study examined one possible factor affecting this variability: whether the perceived quality of parent–child relationships is associated with the number of early memories young adults can retrieve, and their age at the time of their first memory. We found such associations but they were qualified by parent gender. Mother–child relationships that were more affectively intense (greater social support but also more negative interchanges) were associated with recalling more early memories, although paternal companionship was most associated with how early an individual's first memory was. Affective tone of retrieved memories was also assessed, and a greater proportion of affectively positive memories (as well as fewer affectively neutral memories for males) was associated with high parental involvement in children's lives. 相似文献
326.
Nicotinic cholinergic synaptic mechanisms in the ventral tegmental area contribute to nicotine addiction 总被引:16,自引:0,他引:16
下载免费PDF全文

Pidoplichko VI Noguchi J Areola OO Liang Y Peterson J Zhang T Dani JA 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2004,11(1):60-69
Tobacco use is a major health problem that is estimated to cause 4 million deaths a year worldwide. Nicotine is the main addictive component of tobacco. It acts as an agonist to activate and desensitize nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs). A component of nicotine's addictive power is attributable to actions on the mesolimbic dopaminergic system, which serves a fundamental role in the acquisition of behaviors that are inappropriately reinforced by addictive drugs. Here we show that nicotine, in the same concentration and time ranges as obtained from tobacco, has three main actions that regulate the activity of midbrain dopamine (DA) neurons. Nicotine first activates and then desensitizes nAChRs on the DA neurons. This process directly excites the DA neurons for a short period of time before the nAChRs desensitize. Nicotine also enhances glutamatergic excitation and decreases GABAergic inhibition onto DA neurons. These events increase the probability for synaptic plasticity, such as long-term potentiation. The short-lived direct excitation of the DA neurons coupled with the enhanced glutamatergic afferent activity provides the presynaptic and postsynaptic coincidence necessary to initiate synaptic potentiation. In total, these synaptic events lead to a relatively long-lasting heightened activity of midbrain DA neurons. Consistent with other summarized studies, this work indicates that the synaptic changes normally associated with learning and memory can be influenced and commandeered during the nicotine addiction process. 相似文献
327.
In a previous study, search for a notched-disk target abutting a square among complete-disk nontargets and squares was inefficient in 250-ms exposures, but relatively efficient in 100-ms exposures. This finding was interpreted as evidence that amodal completion proceeds through a mosaic and then a completion stage, with the latter preempting the former. We used the same target but changed its context: Nontargets were instead notched disks near squares. Task set was also different: Participants searched for a complete disk. Contrary to the prediction of the preemption model, search was efficient in the 100-ms condition and inefficient in the 250-ms condition. We propose that in both the present and the previous studies, the target was ambiguous, and task set and context affected how it was perceived. In both experiments, set effects were evident for 100-ms exposures; context effects were evident for 250-ms exposures. 相似文献
328.
Ann F. Haynos Linsey M. Utzinger Jason M. Lavender Ross D. Crosby Li Cao Carol B. Peterson Scott J. Crow Stephen A. Wonderlich Scott G. Engel James E. Mitchell Daniel Le Grange Andrea B. Goldschmidt 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2018,40(4):691-700
Perfectionism is hypothesized to contribute to the etiology of anorexia nervosa (AN). However, there is little research regarding whether individuals with AN can be classified according to maladaptive (e.g., evaluative concerns) and adaptive (e.g., high personal standards) facets of perfectionism that predict distinct outcomes and might warrant different intervention approaches. In this study, a latent profile analysis was conducted using data from adults with AN (n?=?118). Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (Frost et al. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14(5), 449–46, 1990) subscales were used to identify subgroups differing according to endorsed perfectionism features (e.g., adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism). Generalized linear models were used to compare subgroups on eating disorder and affective symptoms measured through questionnaire and ecological momentary assessment. Four subgroups were identified: (a) Low Perfectionism; (b) High Adaptive and Maladaptive Perfectionism; (c) Moderate Maladaptive Perfectionism; and (d) High Maladaptive Perfectionism. Subgroups differed on overall eating disorder symptoms (p?<?.001), purging (p?=?.005), restrictive eating (p?<?.001), and body checking (p?<?.001) frequency, depressive (p?<?.001) and anxiety (p?<?.001) symptoms, and negative (p?=?.001) and positive (p?<?.001) affect. The Low Perfectionism group displayed the most adaptive scores and the Moderate and High Maladaptive Perfectionism groups demonstrated the most elevated clinical symptoms. The High Adaptive and Maladaptive Perfectionism group demonstrated low affective disturbances, but elevated eating disorder symptoms. Results support the clinical significance of subtyping according to perfectionism dimensions in AN. Research is needed to determine if perfectionism subtyping can enhance individualized treatment targeting in AN. 相似文献
329.
Investigations of childhood amnesia have almost exclusively focused on the earliest memories of adults. Here we investigate the earliest memories of children of 6–19 years old. Parents confirmed the memory events and dated the memories. There were surprisingly few developmental differences between the earliest memories of children. Although 6–9-year-olds recalled earlier events than did older children, there were no differences between older age groups. Memories from all age groups were similar in structure, social orientation, and the nature of the recalled event. However, memories of older children were more likely to involve negative affect. There were also few gender differences, although girls were more likely to recall traumatic or transitional events while boys were more likely to recall play events. Overall, results deepen the paradox of early memory: 6–9-year-olds have verbally accessible memories from very early childhood that then seem to disappear as they get older. 相似文献
330.
This article seeks to contribute to the discussion on the nature of choice in virtue theory. If several different actions
are available to the virtuous agent, they are also likely to vary in their degree of virtue, at least in some situations.
Yet, it is widely agreed that once an action is recognised as virtuous there is no higher level of virtue. In this paper we
discuss how the virtue theorist could accommodate both these seemingly conflicting ideas. We discuss this issue from a modern
Aristotelian perspective, as opposed to a purely exegetic one. We propose a way of resolving what seems to be a major clash
between two central features of virtue ethics. Our proposal is based on the notion of parity, a concept which recently has
received considerable attention in the literature on axiology. Briefly put, two alternatives are on a par (or are ‘roughly
equal’) if they are comparable, although it is not the case that one is better than the other, nor that they are equally good.
The advantages of applying the concept of parity to our problem are twofold. Firstly, it sheds new light on the account of
choice in virtue theory. Secondly, some of the criticisms that have been mounted against the possibility of parity can be
countered by considering the notion of choice from a virtue theory perspective. 相似文献