首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   12451篇
  免费   647篇
  国内免费   4篇
  2021年   103篇
  2020年   182篇
  2019年   219篇
  2018年   286篇
  2017年   332篇
  2016年   336篇
  2015年   259篇
  2014年   297篇
  2013年   1272篇
  2012年   509篇
  2011年   545篇
  2010年   338篇
  2009年   341篇
  2008年   412篇
  2007年   428篇
  2006年   381篇
  2005年   348篇
  2004年   341篇
  2003年   310篇
  2002年   327篇
  2001年   303篇
  2000年   303篇
  1999年   258篇
  1998年   146篇
  1997年   126篇
  1996年   142篇
  1995年   121篇
  1994年   112篇
  1993年   115篇
  1992年   192篇
  1991年   173篇
  1990年   157篇
  1989年   144篇
  1988年   137篇
  1987年   135篇
  1986年   144篇
  1985年   165篇
  1984年   149篇
  1983年   120篇
  1982年   122篇
  1981年   126篇
  1980年   108篇
  1979年   160篇
  1977年   115篇
  1976年   100篇
  1975年   122篇
  1974年   139篇
  1973年   136篇
  1972年   110篇
  1971年   100篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
241.
242.
243.
244.
245.
246.
247.
248.
Paternalism in family caregiving may jeopardize the older persons' autonomy; it needs to be better understood. Study objectives were to determine the relationship of belief in paternalism to personal-social characteristics and to determine the relative importance of these variables as predictors of belief in paternalism. Forty-six pairs of daughters (age 49.7) and mothers (age 77.7) were measured on belief in paternalism, dogmatism, attitude toward elders, affective feelings toward the other, and background and caregiving variables. Among both mothers and daughters, dogmatism and attitude toward elders were related to belief in paternalism; daughters' affective feelings was also related. Caregiving variables were unrelated, and demographic background was important only for daughters. Attitude toward elders was the strongest predictor. Results were interpreted in terms of a traditional family ideology.  相似文献   
249.
The goal of the research was to compare decisions under risk in a situation in which forecasters (F) communicate to decision makers (DM) either numerically (e.g., .70) or verbally (e.g., likely) about the chances that a binary event will occur. Following each forecast, the DM bid for a winning or losing lottery based on the event. In Experiment 1 Fs and DMs also provided numerical translations of each verbal forecast after the DMs' bid. In Experiment 2 the DMs provided membership functions over the [0, 1] interval for each phrase used by the Fs. The primary results were: (a) extreme similarity in the DM's bids and rates of bidding under the two modes of communication; (b) greater variability in bids to specific verbal than numerical forecasts; (c) a pattern of bids, in which DMs demonstrated risk seeking for gains and risk neutrality for losses; (d) DMs' numerical translations in Experiment 1 were closer to .50 than were those of Fs; and (e) phrases selected by Fs had high membership values to DMs for the probabilities the Fs were attempting to describe. Points (a), (b), (d), and (e) are consistent with the ν-μ model which assumes that the vague meaning of a probability phrase can be represented by a membership function over the [0, 1] interval, and that in reaching a decision the DM focuses on a range of probabilities with sufficiently high membership. Point (c) is speculatively attributed to social aspects of the dyadic situation, and requires further investigation.  相似文献   
250.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号