首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6778篇
  免费   411篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2021年   61篇
  2020年   140篇
  2019年   145篇
  2018年   197篇
  2017年   234篇
  2016年   244篇
  2015年   163篇
  2014年   198篇
  2013年   781篇
  2012年   321篇
  2011年   339篇
  2010年   215篇
  2009年   225篇
  2008年   276篇
  2007年   295篇
  2006年   251篇
  2005年   242篇
  2004年   234篇
  2003年   207篇
  2002年   217篇
  2001年   109篇
  2000年   115篇
  1999年   110篇
  1998年   95篇
  1997年   87篇
  1996年   97篇
  1995年   88篇
  1994年   78篇
  1993年   65篇
  1992年   82篇
  1991年   65篇
  1990年   55篇
  1989年   53篇
  1988年   50篇
  1987年   43篇
  1986年   47篇
  1985年   67篇
  1984年   61篇
  1983年   55篇
  1982年   78篇
  1981年   73篇
  1980年   55篇
  1979年   57篇
  1978年   45篇
  1977年   71篇
  1976年   42篇
  1975年   50篇
  1974年   43篇
  1973年   47篇
  1972年   37篇
排序方式: 共有7191条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
A three-stage model of the relationships among achievement outcomes, outcome-related affect, attribution, and emotion is tested in two studies. It is suggested that success and failure elicit positive and negative affective states due to prior conditioning. These affective states then lead to an attribution process that serves to defend and enhance self-esteem. Next, emotional labels are chosen that are consistent with the affective states and the attributions. Two studies were designed to test the proposed relationships among achievement outcomes, affective states, and attributions. In the first study, subjects received information indicating that they were strongly or mildly aroused as a result of receiving outcome feedback on an achievement task. The results indicated that low arousal reduced egotistical attributions to internal factors. In the second study, subjects either succeeded or failed on an achievement task. Half of the subjects were provided with an opportunity to misattribute the arousal elicited by their outcomes to an irrelevant source. Subjects in the misattribution condition made less egotistical attributions to external factors than subjects who were given no opportunity to misattribute their arousal. The results of both studies suggest that outcome-related affect mediates the relationship between outcomes and attributions in achievement situations.  相似文献   
192.
193.
L K Tyler 《Cognition》1983,13(3):309-341
The present research focuses on how children integrate the antecedent of different kinds of anaphor into their on-going interpretation of an utterance, and on the kinds of cues they use to help them to do this. These issues were studied by examining the on-line processing of three types of anaphoric devices—repeated noun phrases, general terms and pronoun anaphors. The data showed that by the age of five, anaphoric mapping processes in general are well-mastered, although all age-groups (5, 7, 10 year olds and adults) found general term anaphors more difficult to interpret. The major developmental differences concerned the processing of anaphoric pronouns. For five year olds, pronouns were primarily interpreted as devices which maintained the thematic subject of the discourse, but when there was no thematic subject they relied primarily on pragmatic plausibility in their assignment of pronominal co-reference. As children get older, they are able to take advantage of the lexical properties of pronouns and all three sources of information—lexical, pragmatic inference and the thematic structure of the discourse—play contributory roles in the assignment of reference to a pronoun.  相似文献   
194.
Reports were obtained from middle-class and working-class unemployed British men about change or lack of change since job loss in 37 different behaviors. The behaviors were grouped into Domestic Work, Domestic Pastimes, Other Pastimes, Book Reading, Recreations, Entertainment Through Money, and Social Contacts. Contrary to findings from 1930s research, significant increases in social contact and book reading were reported since job loss. Other significant increases were reported within all categories except Entertainment Through Money, where a significant decline was recorded. A number of social class differences were found. For instance, middleclass unemploycd men were significantly more likely than their working-class counterparts to report increased book reading for study and more visits to the public library. Working-class respondents were significantly more likely to report increased looking around shops without buying and reduced visits to the pub. Several significant associations between reported behavior changes and aspects of psychological and general health were observed.  相似文献   
195.
In an earlier note, a new metric for bounded response scales (MBR) was introduced which resembles the city-block metric but is bounded above. It was suggested the MBR may be more appropriate than minkowski metrics for data obtained with bounded response scales. In this article, some formal properties of the MBR are investigated and it is shown that it is indeed a metric. Empirical predictions are then derived from the MBR and contrasted with those of a “monotonicity hypothesis,” which holds that dissimilarity judgements tend to be biased towards overestimation of larger distances, and with the predictions of the minkowski metrics, which imply additivity of collinear segments. Some empirical results are presented which contradict the monotonicity hypothesis and the minkowski metrics, and favor the MBR. Finally, the logic used to motivate the MBR is invoked to define a subadditive concatenation for bounded norms in the one-dimensional case, which may be useful in psychophysical work where the upper bounds are often real, rather than due to the response scale. This concatenation predicts understimation for doubling and overestimation for halving and middling tasks.  相似文献   
196.
Electrographic (EEG, EOG, EMG) indices have been used for some decades in the definition of the stages of sleep and more recently in the diagnosis of sleep-related disorders, e.g., insomnia, despite the lack of detailed information concerning the precise relationship between such electrographic indices and behavioral and subjective criteria of sleep. Evaluation of the relationship between EEG, behavioral, and self-report measures of sleep onset latency and sleep duration was conducted using 17 young normal sleepers. The behavioral measure was provided by an apparatus which records subjects' button-press responses to an auditory stimulus presented at various interstimulus intervals (2, 5, and random 1, 2, 5, 7, and 10 min). The behavioral and stage 2 EEG estimates of sleep onset latency (SOL) and sleep duration (SD) were almost identical. The stage 1 EEG provided the shortest estimate of SOL; the self-report measure, the longest. The SD measures were in reverse order. There were no significant differences among the three interstimulus interval conditions (2, 5, and random min). Average response rates to the 50-dB chime were 100% during wakefulness, 81% during EEG stage 1, and 8% during EEG stage 2. Almost all stage 2 responding occurred during the first 5 min of each stage 2 period. The validity of electrographic indices as sleep criteria and the implications of the findings for the formulation of an adequate definition of sleep and its clinical measurement are discussed.  相似文献   
197.
The Gestalt studies demonstrated the tendency to visually organize dots on the basis of similarity, proximity, and global properties such as closure, good continuation, and symmetry. The particular organization imposed on a collection of dots is thus determined by many factors, some local, some global. We discuss computational reasons for expecting the initial stages of grouping to be achieved by processes with purely local support. In the case of dot patterns, the expectation is that neighboring dots are grouped as a function of proximity and similarity of contrast, by processes that are independent of the overall organization and the various global factors. We describe experiments that suggest a purely local relationship between proximity and brightness similarity in perceptual grouping.  相似文献   
198.
Nondeterministic programs occurring in recently developed programming languages define nondeterminate partial functions. Formulas (Boolean expressions) of such nondeterministic languages are interpreted by a nonempty subset of {T (“true”), F (“false”), U (“undefined)}. As a semantic basis for the propositional part of a corresponding nondeterministic three-valued logic we study the notion of a truth-function over {T, F, U} which is computable by a nondeterministic evaluation procedure. The main result is that these truth-functions are precisely the functions satisfying four basic properties, called \( \subseteq \) -isotonic, \( \subseteq \) ?-isotonic, hereditarily guarded, and hereditarily guard-using, and that a function satisfies these properties iff it is explicitly definable (in a certain normal form) from “if..then..else..fi”, binary choice, and constants.  相似文献   
199.
Studied the effects of attitude extremity on perceived consensus and willingness to ascribe trait terms to others with either pro or anti nuclear attitudes. Results showed that attitude extremity affected consensus estimates. Trait attributions revealed a clear effect for valence, especially for the extreme attitude groups. Subjects with extreme attitudes also ascribed more traits to both pro and anti others than subjects with relatively moderate attitudes.  相似文献   
200.
This article discusses the attenuating mechanisms of group psychotherapy that support and enhance analytic work with borderline patients in combined group and individual psychotherapy. A group case study is presented which addresses the manifestations and management of transference and countertransference in a combined treatment approach. Combined treatment was observed to respond effectively to both the intrapsychic and interpersonal difficulties of borderline patients. A multidimensional model for treating these patients in a community mental health setting is proposed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号