全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6635篇 |
免费 | 219篇 |
专业分类
6854篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 57篇 |
2020年 | 113篇 |
2019年 | 133篇 |
2018年 | 171篇 |
2017年 | 217篇 |
2016年 | 216篇 |
2015年 | 160篇 |
2014年 | 188篇 |
2013年 | 743篇 |
2012年 | 304篇 |
2011年 | 328篇 |
2010年 | 210篇 |
2009年 | 214篇 |
2008年 | 266篇 |
2007年 | 282篇 |
2006年 | 239篇 |
2005年 | 234篇 |
2004年 | 227篇 |
2003年 | 201篇 |
2002年 | 213篇 |
2001年 | 104篇 |
2000年 | 113篇 |
1999年 | 107篇 |
1998年 | 92篇 |
1997年 | 85篇 |
1996年 | 94篇 |
1995年 | 87篇 |
1994年 | 78篇 |
1993年 | 62篇 |
1992年 | 81篇 |
1991年 | 61篇 |
1990年 | 52篇 |
1989年 | 51篇 |
1988年 | 51篇 |
1987年 | 42篇 |
1986年 | 46篇 |
1985年 | 63篇 |
1984年 | 59篇 |
1983年 | 51篇 |
1982年 | 72篇 |
1981年 | 71篇 |
1980年 | 52篇 |
1979年 | 52篇 |
1978年 | 44篇 |
1977年 | 66篇 |
1976年 | 40篇 |
1975年 | 48篇 |
1974年 | 40篇 |
1973年 | 45篇 |
1972年 | 35篇 |
排序方式: 共有6854条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
831.
832.
This study investigated the influences of social comparisons on group polarization in decision making among members of selected Kenyan secondary school disciplinary panels. Ten participants from 10 selected secondary school disciplinary panels were involved. Semi-structured interviews were used to collect qualitative data on the experiences of the participants regarding the influences of social comparisons on their decision-making behaviours during participation in their schools’ disciplinary hearing meetings. The results revealed that in the dynamic interactions among disciplinary panel members during disciplinary hearing meetings, the panel members engaged in social comparisons among themselves, leading to their shifts in decision-making behaviours in favour of consensus in group decisions or group polarization in decision making. 相似文献
833.
Alison C. Bowling Peter Lindsay Belinda G. Smith Kerri Storok 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(2):201-219
Older adults appear to have greater difficulty ignoring distractions during day-to-day activities than younger adults. To assess these effects of age, the ability of adults aged between 50 and 80 years to ignore distracting stimuli was measured using the antisaccade and oculomotor capture tasks. In the antisaccade task, observers are instructed to look away from a visual cue, whereas in the oculomotor capture task, observers are instructed to look toward a colored singleton in the presence of a concurrent onset distractor. Index scores of the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) were compared with capture errors, and with prosaccade errors on the antisaccade task. A higher percentage of capture errors were made on the oculomotor capture tasks by the older members of the cohort compared to the younger members. There was a weak relationship between the attention index and capture errors, but the visuospatial/constructional index was the strongest predictor of prosaccade error rate in the antisaccade task. The saccade reaction times (SRTs) of correct initial saccades in the oculomotor capture task were poorly correlated with age, and with the neurospsychological tests, but prosaccade SRTs in both tasks moderately correlated with antisaccade error rate. These results were interpreted in terms of a competitive integration (or race) model. Any variable that reduces the strength of the top-down neural signal to produce a voluntary saccade, or that increases saccade speed, will enhance the likelihood that a reflexive saccade to a stimulus with an abrupt onset will occur. 相似文献
834.
The aim of this paper was to understand why some people experience certain emotions in a specific situation, whereas others do not. We postulate that these individual differences arise from individual differences in two underlying processes of emotion elicitation: (i) individual differences in the emotion components (appraisals and action tendencies) that situations activate in a person and (ii) individual differences in how these emotion components are related to subjective emotional experience. In this paper, we re‐analysed data from two studies on anger to capture the structure of these two types of individual differences by using clustering modelling techniques. Consistent results across the two studies demonstrated that individuals differ in anger because they (i) differ in how easy they experience anger‐relevant appraisals (Studies 1 and 2) and action tendencies (Study 2) in increasingly negative situations and (ii) differ in which of these components are necessary to experience anger. Copyright © 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
835.
Peter Singer 《Journal of applied philosophy》2016,33(1):31-35
In Animal Liberation I argued that we commonly ignore or discount the interests of sentient members of other species merely because they are not human, and that this bias in favour of members of our own species is, in important respects, parallel to the biases that lie behind racism and sexism. Shelly Kagan, in ‘What's Wrong With Speciesism’ misconstrues this argument, as well as the principle of equal consideration of interests, which I offer as an alternative to speciesism. Kagan also offers, as an alternative explanation of, and possible justification for, our discounting the interests of nonhuman animals, the suggestion that your interests count more if you are a member of a species whose typical adult members are persons. Although this view is not a form of speciesism, Kagan seems not to be aware of the fact that it is a view commonly defended by advocates of natural law ethics, on which there is already an extensive critical literature. 相似文献
836.
One major difficulty confronting attempts to clarify the epistemological and ontological status of abstract objects is determining the sense, if any, in which such entities may be characterised as mind and language independent. Our contention is that the tolerant reductionist position of Michael Dummett can be strengthened by drawing on Husserl's mature account of the constitution of ideal objects and mathematical objectivity. According to the Husserlian position we advocate, abstract singular terms pick out weakly mind‐independent sedimented meaning‐contents. These meaning‐contents serve as the ‘thin’ referents of abstract singular terms, but are ultimately founded in prior acts of meaning‐constitution. 相似文献
837.
838.
839.
840.