首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   176433篇
  免费   8033篇
  国内免费   157篇
  184623篇
  2021年   1511篇
  2020年   2815篇
  2019年   3487篇
  2018年   3602篇
  2017年   4083篇
  2016年   4707篇
  2015年   3979篇
  2014年   4859篇
  2013年   23699篇
  2012年   4735篇
  2011年   3920篇
  2010年   3987篇
  2009年   4845篇
  2008年   4022篇
  2007年   3624篇
  2006年   4202篇
  2005年   4166篇
  2004年   3658篇
  2003年   3249篇
  2002年   3106篇
  2001年   3306篇
  2000年   3117篇
  1999年   3134篇
  1998年   2868篇
  1997年   2693篇
  1996年   2605篇
  1995年   2442篇
  1994年   2403篇
  1993年   2348篇
  1992年   2580篇
  1991年   2424篇
  1990年   2275篇
  1989年   2224篇
  1988年   2197篇
  1987年   2225篇
  1986年   2190篇
  1985年   2451篇
  1984年   2545篇
  1983年   2337篇
  1982年   2404篇
  1981年   2371篇
  1980年   2211篇
  1979年   2232篇
  1978年   2202篇
  1977年   2160篇
  1976年   1964篇
  1975年   2011篇
  1974年   2057篇
  1973年   1965篇
  1972年   1541篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
182.
183.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
184.
185.
186.
187.
Drawing on terror management theory (TMT), we discuss the psychological motivations that shape personality at two levels: the characteristically human personality common to us all and the individual differences that distinguish some people from others. TMT posits that the motivation to protect the self against deep-rooted fears about mortality drives people to maintain meaningful, reliable conceptions of reality and positive evaluations of themselves, two broad tendencies that form the foundation of every person’s personality. We review studies showing that mortality reminders increase efforts to bolster cultural sources of meaning and self-esteem in similar ways across individuals and cultures. TMT also posits that individual differences in personality partly reflect the different sources of meaning and self-esteem that people invest in to assuage mortality fears. We review evidence that individual differences predict the degree and direction of people’s defensive responses to mortality reminders. Directions for future research are discussed.  相似文献   
188.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
189.
190.
This study explored the association between interpersonal factors and depressive symptoms in first-time mothers over the first two years of parenthood. An interpersonal style characterized by dependency, recollections of rejection in childhood, and current relationship characteristics was assessed in a nonclinical sample of 133 women. The final model explained 52% of the variance in depressive symptoms at 24 months. Controlling for initial symptom levels, interpersonal variables explained 24% of the outcome variance. The findings suggest that two interpersonal attributes, peer rejection in childhood and a dependent interpersonal style, are particularly important to our understanding of depressive symptomatology.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号