全文获取类型
收费全文 | 29076篇 |
免费 | 657篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
29736篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 308篇 |
2019年 | 401篇 |
2018年 | 540篇 |
2017年 | 611篇 |
2016年 | 635篇 |
2015年 | 484篇 |
2014年 | 544篇 |
2013年 | 2495篇 |
2012年 | 918篇 |
2011年 | 959篇 |
2010年 | 601篇 |
2009年 | 577篇 |
2008年 | 757篇 |
2007年 | 824篇 |
2006年 | 693篇 |
2005年 | 677篇 |
2004年 | 645篇 |
2003年 | 612篇 |
2002年 | 653篇 |
2001年 | 856篇 |
2000年 | 837篇 |
1999年 | 650篇 |
1998年 | 314篇 |
1997年 | 280篇 |
1996年 | 311篇 |
1995年 | 278篇 |
1992年 | 532篇 |
1991年 | 505篇 |
1990年 | 474篇 |
1989年 | 448篇 |
1988年 | 470篇 |
1987年 | 423篇 |
1986年 | 437篇 |
1985年 | 453篇 |
1984年 | 374篇 |
1983年 | 334篇 |
1982年 | 280篇 |
1981年 | 276篇 |
1979年 | 407篇 |
1978年 | 297篇 |
1977年 | 280篇 |
1975年 | 331篇 |
1974年 | 399篇 |
1973年 | 411篇 |
1972年 | 323篇 |
1971年 | 309篇 |
1970年 | 301篇 |
1968年 | 368篇 |
1967年 | 320篇 |
1966年 | 299篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
The peak amplitude of EMG activity was measured from the orbicularis oris superior (OOS), orbicularis oris inferior (OOI) and masseter muscles for 3 normal 4-yr.-old children and compared to past data gathered on a group of articulatory disordered children, normal children, and adults. The 4-yr.-olds evidenced greater average peak EMG activity than the other groups, suggesting that speech-sound production becomes more efficient with maturation. Also, levels of variability obtained for the younger children were considerably smaller than for the normal speakers across the three muscles, showing that motor equivalence changes with age whereby the flexibility in the control of articulators increases. The coefficient of variation for the OOI muscle for the disordered children, however, was similar to that for the 4-yr.-olds, while the corresponding data for the other two muscles paralleled those for normal ones. These findings were interpreted as reflecting a delay in the development of speech-motor equivalence that affects certain muscles while sparing others. 相似文献
162.
This experiment concerned the contribution of polydipsia on the temporal discrimination of rats during a fixed-interval 60-sec. schedule. In this study, the timing accuracy of 12 rats which had access to water during training was compared to that of 12 rats which had no water during training. The rats were trained for 25 sessions on an FI 60-sec. schedule. In early sessions before polydipsia was fully developed, no differences existed between the timing accuracy of the water group and no-water group. As the amount of water drunk by the water group increased as the number of sessions increased, a parallel increase was noted in the timing accuracy of the water group. In the final sessions, a significant difference was found between the timing accuracy of rats in the water group and that of those in the no-water group. It was concluded that polydipsia facilitated the development of the temporal discrimination which is characteristic of a fixed-interval 60-sec. schedule. 相似文献
163.
164.
Some relations between maximum likelihood factor analysis and factor indeterminacy are discussed. Bounds are derived for the minimum average correlation between equivalent sets of correlated factors which depend on the latent roots of the factor intercorrelation matrix . Empirical examples are presented to illustrate some of the theory and indicate the extent to which it can be expected to be relevant in practice. 相似文献
165.
166.
167.
Roger B. Howard 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1972,12(4):327-332
It has been suggested that figural aftereffects, apparent motion, and paracontrast may be produced by very similar or equivalent processes. The empirical functions relating figural aftereffect to the duration of the inspection figure and the interfigural distance are significantly different from the corresponding apparent motion and paracontrast functions. The functions relating paracontrast to the interstimulus interval are significantly different from the corresponding figural-aftereffect and apparent-motion functions. Thus, it is unlikely that the processes underlying the three phenomena are the same. Researchers who attempt to study feature analyzers in the human visual system should consider using more than one dependent variable, since none of the three phenomena contains all of the available information. Within-Ss designs should be avoided because a strong response bias exists between figural-aftereffect and apparent-motion tasks. 相似文献
168.
Leander JD Milan MA Jasper KB Heaton KL 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1972,17(2):229-235
The vocal behavior of three Cebus monkeys was maintained by fixed-ratio schedules of response dependent reinforcement at values between fixed-ratio 1 and fixed-ratio 15. In one monkey that was exposed to variable-interval, fixed-interval, and conjunctive fixed-ratio fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement, vocal responding occurred at a low rate, but schedule-appropriate patterns were maintained. The rates and patterns of responding engendered indicated that the vocal operant can be brought under schedule control in the monkey by the use of response-dependent reinforcement. 相似文献
169.
170.
Steven B. Christopher 《Behavior research methods》1972,4(1):19-20
The validity of a primate dominance hierarchy was examined in terms of its relationship to social interactions. Twelve social behaviors associated with dominance and aggression were recorded during 61 h of observation of six pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina). Of the 1,462 interactions recorded, 94.7% were oriented in the water dominance hierarchy in the direction that would be predicted on the basis of their ascribed functions. 相似文献