全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5989篇 |
免费 | 953篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
6947篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 72篇 |
2018年 | 94篇 |
2017年 | 92篇 |
2016年 | 111篇 |
2015年 | 89篇 |
2014年 | 96篇 |
2013年 | 506篇 |
2012年 | 191篇 |
2011年 | 219篇 |
2010年 | 225篇 |
2009年 | 286篇 |
2008年 | 237篇 |
2007年 | 269篇 |
2006年 | 206篇 |
2005年 | 184篇 |
2004年 | 207篇 |
2003年 | 219篇 |
2002年 | 195篇 |
2001年 | 188篇 |
2000年 | 95篇 |
1999年 | 153篇 |
1998年 | 136篇 |
1997年 | 103篇 |
1996年 | 151篇 |
1995年 | 108篇 |
1994年 | 118篇 |
1993年 | 112篇 |
1992年 | 141篇 |
1991年 | 133篇 |
1990年 | 127篇 |
1989年 | 113篇 |
1988年 | 93篇 |
1987年 | 80篇 |
1986年 | 81篇 |
1985年 | 74篇 |
1984年 | 86篇 |
1983年 | 49篇 |
1982年 | 91篇 |
1981年 | 69篇 |
1980年 | 57篇 |
1979年 | 63篇 |
1976年 | 51篇 |
1973年 | 49篇 |
1972年 | 54篇 |
1971年 | 50篇 |
1968年 | 66篇 |
1966年 | 53篇 |
1964年 | 80篇 |
1963年 | 73篇 |
1962年 | 48篇 |
排序方式: 共有6947条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Susan R. Goldman 《Applied cognitive psychology》1990,4(3):220-222
72.
Critical Flicker Fusion Thresholds (CFFTs) were measured in 644 elderly people identified from community-based general practice
records, and the relationship of CFFT with age was investigated. The CFFT was measured using the method of limits with mean
scores for three ascending (flicker/fusion) and three descending (fusion/flicker) presentations being recorded. The difference
between the ascending and descending means was found to be significantly correlated with age (r=−0.131,P<0.001) and may reflect a reduced sensitivity of the Central Nervous System to suprathreshold flicker with increasing age.
The results suggest that CFFT measurement has an important role to play in gerontological research as an objective measure
of cognitive aspects of the ageing process. 相似文献
73.
74.
Erratum
Overt integrity tests versus personality-based measures of delinquency: An empirical comparis 相似文献75.
This study investigated the psychological and physical health status of 265 crew members on two U.S. Navy ships (a guided
missile cruiser and a frigate on convoy escort duty for tankers under U.S. flag) during the Persian Gulf conflict at four
time periods during combat-ready operations on a single day. Findings suggest that adaptation occurs to extreme stressors
associated with sustained operations. Suggestions are made relative to measures that may minimize psychological and physical
conditions that adversely affect performance under conditions of heavy stress.
Report No. 89-18, supported by the Naval Medical Research and Development Command, Bethesda MD, Department of the Navy, under
research Work Unit No. 63206N.M0096. The opinions expressed in this paper are those of the author and do not reflect the official
policy or position of the Department of the Navy, Department of Defense, nor the U.S. Government. 相似文献
76.
Dr. Susan J. Blackmore Ph.D. 《Journal of Near-Death Studies》1993,11(4):205-217
An advertisement in an Indian newspaper solicited accounts from people who had come close to death. Of 19 received, 7 reported no experiences, 4 reported dreamlike experiences, and 8 reported near-death experiences (NDEs). These DNEs were comparable to those reported by Ray-mond Moody, and included tunnels, dark spaces, and bright lights, contrary to previous reports of Indian cases. Many respondents reported positive life changes regardless of whether or not they had an NDE. 相似文献
77.
Irwin Silverman Susan E. Dickens Marion Eals Judith Fine 《Infant and child development》1993,2(3):135-144
This study emanated from the long-standing question of whether a maternal bonding process similar to other mammalian species occurs in humans. The perceptions of adult adoptees and a matched sample of non-adoptees about their parents' behaviours towards them were compared on an original Parental Solicitude Scale. As hypothesized, birth children perceived their mothers as the more solicitous parent, but ratings of adoptive mothers showed a unilateral decline. Further, the differential ratings for birth and adoptive mothers occurred mostly for the factors labelled benignity vs abuse and tolerance vs control, rather than caretaking vs neglect, which was congruent to reported effects in prior studies of lack of mother-infant contact during the sensitive period for bonding. An alternative explanation of the data was also tested but not substantiated. 相似文献
78.
Freedman (1990) introduced a computer analog of the shuttlebox paradigm and presented results to show that this analog could be used for studying human avoidance conditioning. In the present study, a first experimental phase was conducted to test how the fact that the tone was considered aversive in the instructions and the intensity of this tone affect avoidance behavior. In a transfer-of-control test phase, it was tested as to whether the warning stimulus presented as a cue to the aversive stimulus had acquired either aversive or informative quality. An effect of instructions was observed for both levels of the auditive stimulus, and a stimulus effect was found for those groups that were given the instructions that described the tone as aversive. In the case of subjects who in the first phase achieved a certain learning criterion, it was recorded how often they in the second phase selected a condition in which the warning signal of the first phase was not presented. No transfer of control was observed. Thus, no positive evidence was found indicating that Freedman’s computer analog could be used for studying human avoidance conditioning. 相似文献
79.
In Experiment 1, haptically available object properties that would be diagnostic for constrained common object classification at the basic and subordinate levels were elicited in a questionnaire. The results are considered in terms of the nature of the haptically derived representations of common objects. Initial data are also presented regarding knowledge of the natural co-occurrence of properties in haptic object perception. In Experiment 2, the hand movements executed during haptic classification of manipulable common objects were examined. Manual exploration consisted of a two-stage sequence, an initial generalized "grasp-and -lift" routine, followed by a series of more specialized hand-movement patterns strongly driven by knowledge of the property diagnosticity for the specific object (obtained in Experiment 1). The current results may guide computational models of human haptic object classification and the development of perceptual systems for robots equipped with sensate dextrous hands, capable of intelligent exploration, recognition, and manipulation of concrete objects. 相似文献
80.
Choice between delayed reinforcers and fixed-ratio schedules requiring forceful responding. 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

This experiment measured pigeons' choices between delayed reinforcers and fixed-ratio schedules in which a force of approximately 0.48 N was needed to operate the response key. In ratio-delay conditions, subjects chose between a fixed-ratio schedule and an adjusting delay. The delay was increased or decreased several times a session in order to estimate an indifference point--a delay duration at which the two alternatives were chosen about equally often. Each ratio-delay condition was followed by a delay-delay condition in which subjects chose between the adjusting delay and a variable-time schedule, with the components of this schedule selected to match the ratio completion times of the preceding ratio-delay condition. The adjusting delays at the indifference point were longer when the alternative was a fixed-ratio schedule than when it was a matched variable-time schedule, which indicated a preference for the matched variable-time schedules over the fixed-ratio schedules. This preference increased in a nonlinear manner with increasing ratio size. This nonlinearity was inconsistent with a theory that states that indifference points for both time and ratio schedules can be predicted by multiplying the choice response-reinforcer intervals of the two types of schedules by different multiplicative constants. Two other theories, which predict nonlinear increases in preference for the matched variable-time schedules, are discussed. 相似文献