全文获取类型
收费全文 | 199篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
199篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 4篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 4篇 |
2013年 | 39篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 15篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 7篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 5篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 3篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 3篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1962年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有199条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
Marcus Nyström Richard Andersson Kenneth Holmqvist Joost van de Weijer 《Behavior research methods》2013,45(1):272-288
Recording eye movement data with high quality is often a prerequisite for producing valid and replicable results and for drawing well-founded conclusions about the oculomotor system. Today, many aspects of data quality are often informally discussed among researchers but are very seldom measured, quantified, and reported. Here we systematically investigated how the calibration method, aspects of participants’ eye physiologies, the influences of recording time and gaze direction, and the experience of operators affect the quality of data recorded with a common tower-mounted, video-based eyetracker. We quantified accuracy, precision, and the amount of valid data, and found an increase in data quality when the participant indicated that he or she was looking at a calibration target, as compared to leaving this decision to the operator or the eyetracker software. Moreover, our results provide statistical evidence of how factors such as glasses, contact lenses, eye color, eyelashes, and mascara influence data quality. This method and the results provide eye movement researchers with an understanding of what is required to record high-quality data, as well as providing manufacturers with the knowledge to build better eyetrackers. 相似文献
172.
Ljótsson B Hedman E Lindfors P Hursti T Lindefors N Andersson G Rück C 《Behaviour research and therapy》2011,49(1):58-61
We conducted a follow-up of a previously reported study of internet-delivered cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) for IBS, based on exposure and mindfulness exercises (Ljótsson et al. (2010). Internet-delivered exposure and mindfulness based therapy for irritable bowel syndrome – a randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 531–539). Seventy-five participants from the original sample of 85 (88%) reported follow-up data at 15–18 months (mean 16.4 months) after completing treatment. The follow-up sample included participants from both the original study’s treatment group and waiting list after it had been crossed over to treatment. Intention-to-treat analysis showed that treatment gains were maintained on all outcome measures, including IBS symptoms, quality of life, and anxiety related to gastrointestinal symptoms, with mainly large effect sizes (within-group Cohen’s d = 0.78–1.11). A total of fifty participants (59% of the total original sample; 52% of the original treatment group participants and 65% of the original waiting list participants) reported adequate relief of symptoms. Improvements at follow-up were more pronounced for the participants that had completed the full treatment and maintenance of improvement did not seem to be dependent on further treatment seeking. This study suggests that internet-delivered CBT based on exposure and mindfulness has long-term beneficial effects for IBS-patients. 相似文献
173.
Berger T Caspar F Richardson R Kneubühler B Sutter D Andersson G 《Behaviour research and therapy》2011,(3):158-169
Internet-based self-help for social phobia with minimal therapist support via email have shown efficacy in several controlled trials by independent research teams. The role and necessity of therapist guidance is, however, still largely unclear. The present study compared the benefits of a 10-week web-based unguided self-help treatment for social phobia with the same intervention complemented with minimal, although weekly, therapist support via email. Further, a third treatment arm was included, in which the level of support was flexibly stepped up, from no support to email or telephone contact, on demand of the participants. Eighty-one individuals meeting diagnostic criteria for social phobia were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions. Primary outcome measures were self-report measures of symptoms of social phobia. Secondary outcome measures included symptoms of depression, interpersonal problems, and general symptomatology. Measures were taken at baseline, post-treatment, and at 6-month follow-up. Data from a telephone-administered diagnostic interview conducted at post-treatment were also included. Results showed significant symptom reductions in all three treatment groups with large effect sizes for primary social phobia measures (Cohen’s d = 1.47) and for secondary outcome measures (d = 1.16). No substantial and significant between-groups effects were found on any of the measures (Cohen’s d = 00–.36). Moreover, no difference between the three conditions was found regarding diagnosis-free status, clinically significant change, dropout rates, or adherence measures such as lessons or exercises completed. These findings indicate that Internet-delivered treatment for social phobia is a promising treatment option, whether no support is provided or with two different types of therapist guidance. 相似文献
174.
Baldwin SA Murray DM Shadish WR Pals SL Holland JM Abramowitz JS Andersson G Atkins DC Carlbring P Carroll KM Christensen A Eddington KM Ehlers A Feaster DJ Keijsers GP Koch E Kuyken W Lange A Lincoln T Stephens RS Taylor S Trepka C Watson J 《Cognitive behaviour therapy》2011,40(1):15-33
It is essential that outcome research permit clear conclusions to be drawn about the efficacy of interventions. The common practice of nesting therapists within conditions can pose important methodological challenges that affect interpretation, particularly if the study is not powered to account for the nested design. An obstacle to the optimal design of these studies is the lack of data about the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), which measures the statistical dependencies introduced by nesting. To begin the development of a public database of ICC estimates, the authors investigated ICCs for a variety outcomes reported in 20 psychotherapy outcome studies. The magnitude of the 495 ICC estimates varied widely across measures and studies. The authors provide recommendations regarding how to select and aggregate ICC estimates for power calculations and show how researchers can use ICC estimates to choose the number of patients and therapists that will optimize power. Attention to these recommendations will strengthen the validity of inferences drawn from psychotherapy studies that nest therapists within conditions. 相似文献
175.
Ethical Theory and Moral Practice - 相似文献
176.
177.
Abstract Five district physicians and one registered nurse were compared regarding their effectiveness in diagnosing and treating acute upper respiratory tract infections in patients (n=186) at a Swedish community health centre. Attitudes and views of the patient, or of the accompanying adult when the patient was a child, regarding the consultation itself, as well as more general aspects of health care were investigated (n=99) using a questionnaire. The questionnaire included 42 propositions of a “fuzzy” type to be judged on a 100-point truth-value scale, 18 of them concerning the consultation and 24 the more general aspects. No differences between the physicians and the nurse were found in their efficacy of diagosis and treatment. Subjects' overall attitudes were found to be positive toward both the physicians and the nurse. Results of multivariate analyses of variance performed for the consultation and general aspects of health care items separately and concerning type of persons providing health care (physician/nurse), treatment (antibiotics/no antibiotics) and patient age (child/adult), revealed overall differences in questionnaire response as regards patient age in relation to the consultation items. 相似文献
178.
179.
The purpose of the experiment on collaborative memory was to investigate if the collaborative inhibition is due to collaborating pair's disruption of each others' retrieval strategies (the retrieval strategy disruption hypothesis, RSD). The participants' (N = 36) task was to recall a list of 60 words individually and collaboratively. Retrieval strategies were manipulated by presenting word lists organised either by categories or by country of origin and adoption of retrieval strategies were examined by the adjusted ratio of clustering score. Half of the dyads received word lists organised by the same strategy and half of the dyads received word lists organised by different strategies. The results revealed a main effect of collaboration, i.e., collaborative recalled items were significantly fewer than the sum of the non-redundant individually recalled items. Both conditions (same strategies vs different strategies) suffered to the same extent from collaboration, which did not support the RSD hypothesis. However, focusing on words recalled individually but not collaboratively, dyads with different strategies, as predicted by the RSD, forgot more items during collaboration than did dyads with the same strategy. Additional results suggest that collaborative forgetting is mainly manifested by forgetting of non-overlapping items (as measured by individual recalls). 相似文献
180.
In a face-in-the-crowd setting, the authors examined visual search for photographically reproduced happy, angry, and fearful target faces among neutral distractor faces in 3 separate experiments. Contrary to the hypothesis, happy targets were consistently detected more quickly and accurately than angry and fearful targets, as were directed compared with averted targets. There was no consistent effect of social anxiety. A facial emotion recognition experiment suggested that the happy search advantage could be due to the ease of processing happy faces. In the final experiment with perceptually controlled schematic faces, the authors reported more effective detection of angry than happy faces. This angry advantage was most obvious for highly socially anxious individuals when their social fear was experimentally enhanced. 相似文献