全文获取类型
收费全文 | 703篇 |
免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
709篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 17篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 10篇 |
2014年 | 18篇 |
2013年 | 84篇 |
2012年 | 41篇 |
2011年 | 39篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 22篇 |
2008年 | 32篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 23篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 33篇 |
2003年 | 22篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有709条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
Matthew J. Traxler David P. Corina Jill P. Morford Sarah Hafer Liv J. Hoversten 《Memory & cognition》2014,42(1):97-111
This study was designed to determine the feasibility of using self-paced reading methods to study deaf readers and to assess how deaf readers respond to two syntactic manipulations. Three groups of participants read the test sentences: deaf readers, hearing monolingual English readers, and hearing bilingual readers whose second language was English. In Experiment 1, the participants read sentences containing subject-relative or object-relative clauses. The test sentences contained semantic information that would influence online processing outcomes (Traxler, Morris, & Seely Journal of Memory and Language 47: 69–90, 2002; Traxler, Williams, Blozis, & Morris Journal of Memory and Language 53: 204–224, 2005). All of the participant groups had greater difficulty processing sentences containing object-relative clauses. This difficulty was reduced when helpful semantic cues were present. In Experiment 2, participants read active-voice and passive-voice sentences. The sentences were processed similarly by all three groups. Comprehension accuracy was higher in hearing readers than in deaf readers. Within deaf readers, native signers read the sentences faster and comprehended them to a higher degree than did nonnative signers. These results indicate that self-paced reading is a useful method for studying sentence interpretation among deaf readers. 相似文献
42.
Jill T. Shelton Emily M. Elliott B.D. Hill Matthew R. Calamia Wm. Drew Gouvier 《Intelligence》2009,37(3):283-293
The working memory (WM) construct is conceptualized similarly across domains of psychology, yet the methods used to measure WM function vary widely. The present study examined the relationship between WM measures used in the laboratory and those used in applied settings. A large sample of undergraduates completed three laboratory-based WM measures (operation span, listening span, and n-back), as well as the WM subtests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and the Wechsler Memory Scale-III. Performance on all of the WM subtests of the clinical batteries shared positive correlations with the lab measures; however, the Arithmetic and Spatial Span subtests shared lower correlations than the other WM tests. Factor analyses revealed that a factor comprising scores from the three lab WM measures and the clinical subtest, Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS), provided the best measurement of WM. Additionally, a latent variable approach was taken using fluid intelligence as a criterion construct to further discriminate between the WM tests. The results revealed that the lab measures, along with the LNS task, were the best predictors of fluid abilities. 相似文献
43.
Adults rapidly learn phonotactic constraints from brief production or perception experience. Three experiments asked whether this learning is modality-specific, occurring separately in production and perception, or whether perception transfers to production. Participant pairs took turns repeating syllables in which particular consonants were restricted to particular syllable positions. Speakers’ errors reflected learning of the constraints present in the sequences they produced, regardless of whether their partner produced syllables with the same constraints, or opposing constraints. Although partial transfer could be induced (Experiment 3), simply hearing and encoding syllables produced by others did not affect speech production to the extent that error patterns were altered. Learning of new phonotactic constraints was predominantly restricted to the modality in which those constraints were experienced. 相似文献
44.
Curtis W. Hart Robin Scroggs Claude Barbre Antoinette Goodwin Philip Paris Mark J. Hanson Kathryn Madden Annette G.E. Esser Raymond J. Lawrence Jr. Jilisa Snyder Karen A. Monk George M. Klee Loy McGill Jill Carlen Kirby Thandeka Pamela Davis Barnett Gregory Forte J.J. Haines Barry Ulanov Elizabeth MacDonald 《Journal of religion and health》1997,36(1):81-104
45.
President Obama's election has been construed as a potentially positive force for intergroup relations, but this issue has not been previously addressed experimentally. In experiment 1, conducted 4-5 months after the election, White participants were primed with either President Obama or nature before completing a variety of race-related measures. Results indicated that priming Obama did not influence implicit racial bias or internal motivation to control prejudice. However, consistent with exemplar and symbolic racism theories, participants primed with President Obama expressed greater agreement with the tenets of symbolic racism and were more reluctant to accept the possibility that they personally harbored subtle racial bias. Experiment 2, conducted 21 months after the election, replicated the Obama effects from experiment 1 and showed that priming another Black exemplar (Oprah) also increased symbolic racism. Results suggest that highly successful Black exemplars currently serve as a smokescreen for symbolic and subtle racial biases. 相似文献
46.
Reaching toward an object usually consists of a sequence of elemental actions. Using a reaching task sequence, the authors investigated how task elements of that sequence affected feedforward and feedback components of the reaching phase of the movement. Nine right-handed adults performed, with their dominant and nondominant hands, 4 tasks of different complexities: a simple reaching task; a reach-to-grasp task; a reach-to-grasp and lift object task; and a reach-to-grasp, lift, and place object task. Results showed that in the reach-to-grasp and lift object task more time was allocated to the feedforward component of the reach phase, while latency between the task elements decreased. We also found between-hand differences, supporting previous findings of increased efficiency of processing planning-related information in the preferred hand. The presence of task-related modifications supports the concept of contextual effects when planning a movement. 相似文献
47.
The present study describes preschool read-alouds in terms of the types of texts to which children are exposed. The methods involved analyzing the genre and instructional foci of 426 titles read by 13 teachers throughout an entire academic year. Additionally, associations between teacher characteristics and texts teachers read in their classrooms were examined. Findings indicated that (a) narrative texts were the dominate genre read-aloud; (b) children's exposure to alphabet books, nursery rhymes, books featuring math concepts, and multicultural content occurred at generally low rates; and (c) few significant associations exist between quantity of books read and teacher characteristics. 相似文献
48.
Studies suggest that medical students and physicians have higher rates of anxiety, depression, and suicidal ideation than their peers in the general population. Some authors have suggested that medical culture perpetuates these problems by erecting “barriers to treatment,” preventing students and physicians from getting the help they need. Here, the author begins a broader examination of the potential role of culture by examining the myths and symbols that form the basis for medical culture and the medical self-image. The author argues that a medical self-image based on a de-contextualized medical mythology, the Asclepius myth, results in a sense of professional identity that is unbalanced, dehumanized, and characterized by unattainable expectations. The outward expression of this medical self-image, the medical culture, is often a-relational, unhealthy, stressed, or even toxic. The author suggests some ways of re-modeling medical culture, including its rituals and symbols, and medical education in ways that incorporate what is currently kept in its shadow. 相似文献
49.
Measurement of newcomer socialization: Construct validation of a multidimensional scale 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
A construct validation approach was taken to develop a primary measure of newcomer socialization that addresses shortcomings with a prior scale (Chao, Kelly, Wolf, Klein, & Gardner, 1994). Three separate groups of subject matter experts reviewed items to ensure the content validity of the Newcomer Socialization Questionnaire (NSQ). Studies 1 and 2 examined the NSQ’s psychometric properties using employed students and organizational newcomers as participants, respectively. Results illustrate scale reliability, factor structure, convergent and discriminant validity, and correlations with criterion variables. The NSQ measures three dimensions or domains of newcomer socialization: the organization, the group and the job/task. In addition, both factual knowledge and knowledge of expected role behaviors are assessed within each domain. Thus, the NSQ provides a useful measurement tool for researchers and practitioners interested in examining direct outcomes of being socialized. 相似文献
50.
A socio-ecological approach to explain choice behaviour highlights salient personal and social factors in the context of changing
aspirations and opportunities. The population databases are five cohorts of all school leaver applicants for university places
in a state of Australia and a survey for one cohort. Results highlight common behaviour patterns across cohorts, with diverse
outcomes for specific social groups. Offers of university places relate course preferences and school achievement. In addition,
decisions to apply, accept and enrol vary with social experience by socio-economic indicators, geographic location and school
type. Student explanations for their decisions to defer or let the offer lapse entail particular social factors as well as
personal factors of self concepts, interests and financial needs. Findings contribute to a better understanding of choice
behaviour, with a general model of the personal and social factors that explain diverse pathways to higher education. 相似文献