全文获取类型
收费全文 | 318篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
320篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 2篇 |
2019年 | 2篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 7篇 |
2013年 | 26篇 |
2012年 | 14篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 14篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有320条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
Sound symbolism refers to an association between phonemes and stimuli containing particular perceptual and/or semantic elements (e.g., objects of a certain size or shape). Some of the best-known examples include the mil/mal effect (Sapir, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 225–239, 1929) and the maluma/takete effect (Köhler, 1929). Interest in this topic has been on the rise within psychology, and studies have demonstrated that sound symbolic effects are relevant for many facets of cognition, including language, action, memory, and categorization. Sound symbolism also provides a mechanism by which words’ forms can have nonarbitrary, iconic relationships with their meanings. Although various proposals have been put forth for how phonetic features (both acoustic and articulatory) come to be associated with stimuli, there is as yet no generally agreed-upon explanation. We review five proposals: statistical co-occurrence between phonetic features and associated stimuli in the environment, a shared property among phonetic features and stimuli; neural factors; species-general, evolved associations; and patterns extracted from language. We identify a number of outstanding questions that need to be addressed on this topic and suggest next steps for the field. 相似文献
112.
W. Todd Maddox Brian D. Glass Jeffrey B. O’Brien J. Vincent Filoteo F. Gregory Ashby 《Psychological research》2010,74(2):219-236
The category shift literature suggests that rule-based classification, an important form of explicit learning, is mediated
by two separate learned associations: a stimulus-to-label association that associates stimuli and category labels, and a label-to-response
association that associates category labels and responses. Three experiments investigate whether information–integration classification,
an important form of implicit learning, is also mediated by two separate learned associations. Participants were trained on
a rule-based or an information–integration categorization task and then the association between stimulus and category label,
or between category label and response location was altered. For rule-based categories, and in line with previous research,
breaking the association between stimulus and category label caused more interference than breaking the association between
category label and response location. However, no differences in recovery rate emerged. For information–integration categories,
breaking the association between stimulus and category label caused more interference and led to greater recovery than breaking
the association between category label and response location. These results provide evidence that information–integration
category learning is mediated by separate stimulus-to-label and label-to-response associations. Implications for the neurobiological
basis of these two learned associations are discussed. 相似文献
113.
Kimberly E. DiGiorgio Carol R. Glass Diane B. Arnkoff 《Cognitive and behavioral practice》2010,17(2):213-221
The purpose of this study was to examine how therapists conduct Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT) individual psychotherapy with clients, focusing on clinical factors that could account for decisions regarding modifications of DBT (e.g., client diagnosis, therapist theoretical orientation, and intensity of DBT training). Additionally, the study investigated how therapists integrate DBT into their primary approach to therapy. Greater adherence to the DBT protocol was reported by therapists who described using DBT with a client with a diagnosis of borderline personality disorder. More frequent use of DBT components (i.e., group skills training, consultation teams, and telephone consultation) was reported by therapists who viewed their therapy as being guided by an applied behavior analysis/radical behavioral theoretical orientation and by therapists who had received intensive DBT training. Most therapists reported using DBT skills in their non-DBT work, with non-cognitive-behavioral therapists more likely to introduce mindfulness skills. 相似文献
114.
AbstractBehavioral studies consistently find that subjects move their hand along straight paths despite considerations that suggest reaches should be curved. Literature on this topic makes it clear that the experimentally displayed feedback influences how subjects reach. Could the standard visual feedback, a displayed cursor, explain the lack of path curvature in experimental results? To address this question, we conducted three experiments to examine reach behavior in the absence of the standard visual feedback. In the first experiment, we found significant increases in curvature as visual feedback was progressively extinguished across groups. A second experiment revealed that practiced reaches became curved after the standard visual feedback was removed. A final experiment found that subjects’ reaches made before and after a brief display of visual feedback were similar, indicating a preference for specific curved trajectories. Our results suggest that the consistently straight reaches often observed could be due to a bias to move the displayed cursor straight, which when removed reveal subject-specific preferences for reaches that are often curved. 相似文献
115.
D Luszyk F Eggert L Uharek B Glass W Müller-Ruchholtz R Ferstl 《Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie》1992,39(2):249-262
In an operant conditioning paradigm 4 Balb/c inbred strain mice were conditioned to discriminate between urine samples of two other inbred strains. The purpose was to examine whether or not the known stimulus configuration during training had any effect upon the rate of recognition of other stimuli. On the basis of this procedure, we subsequently investigated the extent to which an allogeneic bone marrow transplantation interferes with the olfactory identity of an individual. For the transplants a strain was inbred which had no contact to the animals tested, a procedure used for the first time here. Furthermore, also for the first time, it was possible to demonstrate in a direct comparison, the role played by donor versus recipient characteristics on the olfactory composition of the recipients odor. 相似文献
116.
The effects of continuous and partial reward on the performance of hyperactive and normal children on a concept identification task were compared. Because reduction in information feedback is usually associated with partial reward schedules, the study was designed to yield information regarding the contribution of reduced feeback to performance in the partial reward condition. Previous findings of a performance deficit in hyperactives under partial reward were replicated. The findings help rule out an information feedback explanation for this deficit. The authors suggest that a motivational factor involving the elicitation of frustration when expected rewards fail to appear may be responsible for the poor performance of hyperactives on the partial schedule.This research was supported by Grant No. 943-03-34 from the Social Sciences Research Fund, McGill University. 相似文献
117.
David S Krantz David C Glass Melvin L Snyder 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1974,10(3):284-300
Two experiments were conducted to examine the relationship between stress level and learned helplessness in human subjects. Experiment I subjected subjects to loud or moderate noise in order to induce differential stress. Half of the subjects within each stress group were unable to escape from a series of noise bursts, whereas the other half could terminate each burst by manipulating appropriate switches. After the pretreatment series, the same noise was again delivered to subjects, all of whom could now escape or avoid noise by making an appropriate response on a shuttle box. Inescapable (helplessness) pretreatment interfered with escape learning in the second (test) phase of the study under both levels of stress. Experiment II was a partial replication of the first study using only loud noise. The interference effect during test trials was greater than in the first study. In addition to these findings, measures of a coronary-prone behavior pattern were related to differential susceptibility to the interference effect under both high stress and moderate stress conditions. These results were interpreted in terms of differing perceptions of threat imposed by lack of environmental control. 相似文献
118.
119.
Marcia M. Grenell Carol R. Glass Kathy S. Katz 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1987,15(1):1-13
This study investigated hyperactive children's peer relations using the framework of social skills theory. Subjects' (15 hyperactive and 15 control boys aged 7 through 11) knowledge of socially appropriate behavior was assessed using the Social Knowledge Interview (Geraci & Asher, 1980). Additionally, performance of social skills with peers was rated by independent judges during free play, a cooperative puzzle task, and a persuasion task. Peers also rated subjects'desirability as partners for work, play, and friendship. Analyses showed that hyperactive subjects had deficits in knowledge of how to maintain relationships and handle interpersonal conflict, and demonstrated more negative behavior in the cooperative puzzle task than did controls. Hyperactive subjects also were rated by judges as less likely to achieve academic success than controls and were rated by peers as less desirable potential work partners in school. Significant correlations between social knowledge and performance with peers were found. It was concluded that hyperactive boys exhibit deficits both in their social knowledge and in their performance of socially skilled behavior.This research was conducted as part of doctoral research by the first author, under the supervision of the second and third authors. We are grateful for the assistance of Diane Arnkoff, who served on the committee, for her helpful comments and suggestions. Thanks also go to Al Farrell for his statistical consultation. We gratefully acknowledge the support and assistance of the Georgetown University Child Development Center, where the data were collected. 相似文献
120.
Subjects were taught two eight-term linear orders of the form “A taller than B taller than C ….,” They were then asked to choose the “taller” term in all possible pairwise combinations within each series, and reaction time was measured for each pair. In addition, subjects performed a further task in which they judged whether or not two terms were adjacent in the ordering. In subsequent sessions, subjects were told that the “shortest” term on one list was taller than the “tallest” term on the other, so that the two lists were merged into a single 16-term series. They were then required to choose the “taller” term for both within-groups and between-groups pairs. Subjects did not appear to use the initial groupings in performing this task, even when given training on differential categorical codes (“tall” vs. “short”) for the two sublists. Rather, subjects in all tasks appeared to represent the items as ordered positions along an internal array, so that comparison times depended largely on the differential discriminability of the item positions. In each task decisions were made more quickly if the terms being compared were near the ends of the ordering, rather than near the middle. 相似文献