全文获取类型
收费全文 | 295篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
299篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 20篇 |
2017年 | 13篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 14篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 43篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 29篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 5篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有299条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
Jos Pedro Zúquete 《Religion Compass》2013,7(7):263-271
The sacralization of politics is commonly studied in terms of movements and regimes of the past. This article explains and develops the political–religious concept of missionary politics as an analytical tool to understand political activism in the twenty‐first century. Missionary Politics is a political religion that has at its center a charismatic leadership, a narrative of salvation, ritualization, and the creation of a moral community invested with a collective mission of combating conspiratorial enemies and redeeming the community from its putative crisis. Ongoing research regarding missionary politics demonstrates the continuing importance of the sacralization of the political, which manifests itself in many ways, in contemporary times. 相似文献
152.
153.
Pedro Palacios Cintia Rodríguez Cecilia Méndez-Sánchez Alicia-Edith Hermosillo-De-La-Torre Miguel-Ángel Sahagún Karina Cárdenas 《Estudios de Psicología》2016,37(1):59-89
This paper longitudinally studies the emergence and evolution of the first symbolic uses of objects in Mexican children. We observed eight children in triadic interaction with one of their parents and 10 objects in a semi-structured situation at nine, 12, 15 and 18 months old. The children began to use objects symbolically at 12 months, and the duration and frequency increased with age. The highest percentage of the total frequency of symbolic uses, of the four levels identified, was level 1. The frequency of level 4, where two or more symbolic actions occur one after the other, giving rise to ‘symbolic narratives’, increased according to age. These data confirm that knowledge of the rules of canonical uses of objects are the meanings which first symbols base themselves on and that children use the symbolic uses that they are aware of to create ‘symbolic narratives’. 相似文献
154.
Fernando Rubio‐Garay Miguel A. Carrasco Pedro J. Amor 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2016,57(2):129-135
This study examines how the mechanisms underlying moral disengagement serve as a mediator between anger and hostility and physical and verbal aggression. The study was carried out on 424 participants (61.1% females), aged 15 to 25 years, assessing the direct and indirect effects of the distinct variables using a hierarchical multiple regression analysis and structural equation modeling. The findings suggest that anger and hostility contribute independently and positively to physical and verbal aggression. Moreover, the relationships between anger, hostility, and aggression appear to be mediated by moral disengagement. Indeed, this process of mediation was invariant across sexes, and it tended to be stronger for physical – as opposed to verbal – aggression. 相似文献
155.
Collado Mateo MJ Díaz-Morales JF Escribano Barreno C Delgado Prieto P Randler C 《Psicothema》2012,24(3):410-415
Previous research has indicated the need to use large samples in different cultural contexts in order to clarify age and gender differences on morningness-eveningness and sleep habits. The goal of our research was to study the relationship between morningness-eveningness and sleep habits in a large sample of 2,649 adolescents between 12 and 16 years. The Morningness- Eveningness Scale for Children and an adaptation of the School Sleep Habits Survey measures were used. Results indicated a greater tendency toward eveningness with age and higher eveningness in 13- and 14-year-old girls. Older adolescents claimed later rising time on weekends, later bedtime and shorter sleep length, and greater social jetlag, weekend rise time delay, and weekend bedtime delay. Girls reported earlier rising time on weekdays, later rising time on weekends, longer sleep length on weekends, and greater social jetlag and weekend rising time delay. Lastly, evening oriented adolescents claimed later rising time and bedtime, shorter sleep length on weekdays but longer sleep duration on weekends, and greater social jetlag, weekend rising time delay, and weekend bedtime delay. 相似文献
156.
Pedro J. Boschán 《International Forum of Psychoanalysis》2013,22(4):257-262
This paper considers the fundamental change introduced by Ferenczi in 1919 by proposing the use of countertransference as an instrument. Basically it reconsiders the concept of analytic neutrality; mastery of countertransference is reached through tolerating it, overcoming resistances against it, demanding a very intense involvement of the analyst, as opposed to the image of the surgeon or the mirror. The paper analyzes the implications of this position for psychoanalysis. It places these concepts in the scientific and personal context in which it was written, then follows the later developments of these ideas in Ferenczi's own work, as well as in that of some other authors, and comments on the effects of these ideas on the psychoanalytic movement. Finally it poses some questions regarding our present use of countertransference in clinical work, with two brief vignettes highlighting these points. 相似文献
157.
The purpose of the study was to confirm a model which proposed two basic dimensions in the subjective experience of guilt, one anxious-aggressive and the other empathic, as well as another dimension associated but not intrinsic to it, namely, the associated negative emotions dimension. Participants were 360 adolescents, young adults and adults of both sexes. They were asked to relate one of the situations that most frequently caused them to experience feelings of guilt and to specify its intensity and that of 9 other emotions that they may have experienced, to a greater or lesser extent, at the same time on a 7-point scale. The proposed model was shown to adequately fit the data and to be better than other alternative nested models. This result supports the views of both Freud and Hoffman regarding the nature of guilt, contradictory only at a first glance. 相似文献
158.
Meseguer de Pedro M Soler Sánchez MI Sáez Navarro MC García Izquierdo M 《The Spanish journal of psychology》2008,11(1):219-227
In this work, we analyze various consequences of the phenomenon of mobbing on the health of a work sector with special characteristics: the agro fruit sector. For this purpose, we collected data from a sample of 396 workers (61 men and 331 women) belonging to this sector in the Region of Murcia (Spain). A questionnaire with the following measurement instruments was administered: a Spanish adaptation of the revised Negative Acts Questionnaire (Sáez, García, & Llor, 2003), the Psychosomatic Problems Questionnaire (Hock, 1988), and a measure of absenteeism. The results revealed a significant and positive relation between workplace mobbing and psychosomatic symptoms, but not with absenteeism. The implications of the results for future research are discussed. 相似文献
159.
Fernández-Abascal EG Guerra P Martínez F Domínguez FJ Muñoz MA Egea DA Martín MD Mata JL Rodríguez S Vila J 《Psicothema》2008,20(1):104-113
The International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS): Spanish Norms. The Spanish norms for the International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS) are reported. The IADS is a standardized set of 110 digitalized sounds which are both reliable and valid for the experimental study of emotional processes. The construction of the IADS is based on Peter J. Lang's dimensional model of emotions. Participants were 1.716 university students (1.136 women) who assessed the sounds using the three scales of the Self-Assessment Manikin: affective valence, arousal, and dominance. The results show a distribution of the sounds, within the bidimensional plot defined by valence and arousal, similar to the North-American student population with a typical boomerang shape. No significant differences were found when comparing men and women nor when comparing the Spanish and North-American samples in the total set of sounds. The results are discussed in relation to the International Affective Picture System (IAPS). The norms for each of the 110 sounds are presented in an annexed table. 相似文献
160.
Ortiz Barón MJ Apocada Urquijo P Etxebarria Bilbao I Fuentes Rebollo MJ López Sánchez F 《Psicothema》2008,20(4):712-717
This study aimed to analyze the relationship between parents' affection, acceptance, emotional communication, the transmission of values and moral intervention, on the one hand, and children's moral emotions and behaviors on the other (empathy, guilt, internalized behavior and reparation). It also aimed to identify the family variables that influence moral internalization in children. The sample comprised 485 children (244 boys and 241 girls) aged between 6 and 8. The results showed that the principal variable to predict internalized behavior in girls was maternal affection, whereas for boys, the principal variables were maternal emotional communication and transmission of values. The family variables that explain moral internalization in extreme groups of children are basically parents' affection, emotional communication, the transmission of values and moral intervention. 相似文献