首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   172774篇
  免费   7798篇
  国内免费   157篇
  2021年   1508篇
  2020年   2805篇
  2019年   3484篇
  2018年   3517篇
  2017年   3953篇
  2016年   4639篇
  2015年   3956篇
  2014年   4841篇
  2013年   23721篇
  2012年   4509篇
  2011年   3552篇
  2010年   3896篇
  2009年   4798篇
  2008年   3868篇
  2007年   3380篇
  2006年   4015篇
  2005年   4010篇
  2004年   3496篇
  2003年   3174篇
  2002年   2938篇
  2001年   3019篇
  2000年   2914篇
  1999年   3004篇
  1998年   2845篇
  1997年   2674篇
  1996年   2609篇
  1995年   2436篇
  1994年   2386篇
  1993年   2330篇
  1992年   2516篇
  1991年   2363篇
  1990年   2223篇
  1989年   2155篇
  1988年   2141篇
  1987年   2152篇
  1986年   2141篇
  1985年   2341篇
  1984年   2515篇
  1983年   2291篇
  1982年   2381篇
  1981年   2337篇
  1980年   2191篇
  1979年   2158篇
  1978年   2176篇
  1977年   2138篇
  1976年   1952篇
  1975年   1978篇
  1974年   2076篇
  1973年   1942篇
  1972年   1500篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
191.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication.  相似文献   
192.
193.
Do the conditions under which promises are made determine whether they ought to be kept? Philosophers have placed a number of conditions on promising which, they hold, must be met in order to make promise-keeping obligatory. In so doing, they have distinguished valid promises from invalid promises and justified promises from promises that are not justified. Considering such conditions, one by one, we argue that they are mistaken. In the first place, the conditions they lay down are not necessary for either valid or justified promise-making. In the second place, promises need not meet such conditions in order to create moral obligations. In general, such analyses of promising fail because they suffer from a confusion between promise-making and promise-keeping. Philosophers have wrongly supposed that obligations to keep promises are dependent upon, or derivable from, the quality of the promises themselves, at the time they are made, instead of focusing on conditions that must be satisfied at the time when promises are supposed to be kept. It is not the quality of a promise that determines an obligation to keep it but the rightness or wrongness of performing the promised act.  相似文献   
194.
Ohne ZusammenfassungIn dankbarer Erinnerung an meinen verehrten Münchener Lehrer Kurt Huber, dessen frühzeitigen gewaltsamen Tod (13. 7. 43) wir noch immer beklagen. Die Arbeit entsprang einer seiner letzten wissenschaftlichen Anregungen.  相似文献   
195.
196.
Wray  K. Brad 《Synthese》2019,196(3):819-832
Synthese - Hoyningen-Huene (Systematicity: the nature of science, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2013) develops an account of what science is, distinguishing it from common sense. According to...  相似文献   
197.
198.
199.
200.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号