首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   31187篇
  免费   1166篇
  国内免费   11篇
  2019年   334篇
  2018年   471篇
  2017年   518篇
  2016年   526篇
  2015年   403篇
  2014年   500篇
  2013年   2576篇
  2012年   845篇
  2011年   887篇
  2010年   587篇
  2009年   582篇
  2008年   874篇
  2007年   807篇
  2006年   756篇
  2005年   701篇
  2004年   697篇
  2003年   671篇
  2002年   683篇
  2001年   735篇
  2000年   731篇
  1999年   606篇
  1998年   375篇
  1997年   352篇
  1996年   349篇
  1995年   343篇
  1992年   552篇
  1991年   513篇
  1990年   498篇
  1989年   476篇
  1988年   492篇
  1987年   455篇
  1986年   497篇
  1985年   498篇
  1984年   444篇
  1983年   392篇
  1981年   317篇
  1979年   437篇
  1978年   382篇
  1977年   335篇
  1976年   348篇
  1975年   390篇
  1974年   481篇
  1973年   503篇
  1972年   372篇
  1971年   392篇
  1970年   352篇
  1969年   381篇
  1968年   459篇
  1967年   422篇
  1966年   446篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
41.
Four experiments were conducted to determine whether echoic memory plays a role in differences between good and poor readers. The first two experiments used a suffix procedure in which the subject is read a list of digits with either a tone control or the word go appended to the list. For lists that exceeded the length of the subjects memory span by one digit (i.e., that avoided ceiling effects), the poor readers showed a larger decrement in the suffix condition than did the good readers. The third experiment was directed at the question of whether the duration of echoic memory is different for good and poor readers. Children shadowed words presented to one ear at a rate determined to give 75-85% shadowing accuracy. The items presented to the nonattended ear were words and an occasional digit. At various intervals after the presentation of the digit, a light signaled that the subject was to cease shadowing and attempt to recall any digit that had occurred in the nonattended ear recently. Whereas good and poor readers recalled the digit equally if tested immediately after presentation, the poor readers showed a faster decline in recall of the digit as retention interval increased. A fourth experiment was conducted to determine whether the differences in echoic memory were specific to speech stimuli or occurred at a more basic level of aural persistence. Bursts of white noise were separated by 9-400 ms of silence and the subject was to say whether there were one or two sounds presented. There were no differences in detectability functions for good and poor readers.  相似文献   
42.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号